29.10.2014 Views

Educational Research - the Ethics and Aesthetics of Statistics

Educational Research - the Ethics and Aesthetics of Statistics

Educational Research - the Ethics and Aesthetics of Statistics

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

6 n = 1: The Science <strong>and</strong> Art <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Single Case in <strong>Educational</strong> <strong>Research</strong> 81<br />

that describes <strong>the</strong> long-term stability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mean <strong>of</strong> a r<strong>and</strong>om variable. Basically,<br />

<strong>the</strong> law dictates that <strong>the</strong> more observations you make <strong>of</strong> a particular variable, <strong>the</strong><br />

more <strong>the</strong> sample mean will tend to approach <strong>and</strong> stay close to a particular value. If,<br />

for example, we toss a (fair) coin just two or three times, <strong>the</strong>n it is quite possible<br />

that it will l<strong>and</strong> as heads every time; if we continue for 10 or 20 tosses, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> proportion<br />

<strong>of</strong> heads will start to approximate to 50%; <strong>and</strong> it will get closer <strong>and</strong> closer<br />

to 50% <strong>the</strong> more times you toss <strong>the</strong> coin.<br />

This law <strong>of</strong> large numbers tends to be extended (perhaps incorrectly, since it<br />

is essentially a principle applicable to a r<strong>and</strong>om variable with a finite expected<br />

value) as a general principle <strong>of</strong> research based on quantitative measure to indicate<br />

that, o<strong>the</strong>r things being equal, <strong>the</strong> larger <strong>the</strong> sample, <strong>the</strong> closer to a true picture,<br />

i.e. <strong>the</strong> more valid, will be <strong>the</strong> results. “The larger <strong>the</strong> sample size, <strong>the</strong> greater its<br />

accuracy... The sampling error – <strong>the</strong> difference between <strong>the</strong> sample <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> population<br />

which are due for sampling – can be reduced by increasing sampling size”<br />

[though] “after a certain level, increases in accuracy tend to trail <strong>of</strong>f as sample size<br />

increases” (Bryman & Cramer, 1990, p. 104). This principle applies in general to<br />

both correlational research (e.g. To what extent is entry to higher education a function<br />

<strong>of</strong> ethnicity, gender or social class?) <strong>and</strong> experimental research (How effective<br />

are mentoring schemes aimed at under-represented groups in raising entry to higher<br />

education in <strong>the</strong>se groups?) In any research which is looking at <strong>the</strong> relationship<br />

between different variables, for example, if <strong>the</strong>re are very few observations, <strong>the</strong>n<br />

<strong>the</strong>re are also few possible combinations <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> values <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> variables, <strong>and</strong> thus <strong>the</strong><br />

probability <strong>of</strong> obtaining by chance a combination <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> values indicative <strong>of</strong> a strong<br />

relation is relatively high. It is a feature <strong>of</strong> such research that when <strong>the</strong> enquiry has<br />

to be based on a sample ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> entire relevant population, we are never<strong>the</strong>less<br />

interested in <strong>the</strong> confidence with which we can extrapolate from <strong>the</strong> sample to<br />

that whole population – <strong>and</strong>, o<strong>the</strong>r things being equal, a larger sample reduces <strong>the</strong><br />

risk <strong>of</strong> error <strong>and</strong> or pure chance <strong>and</strong> increases <strong>the</strong> confidence with which one can<br />

draw inferences from <strong>the</strong> sample to <strong>the</strong> whole population.<br />

It is against <strong>the</strong>se sorts <strong>of</strong> expectations from quantitative research traditions that<br />

a sample size <strong>of</strong> a single case (n = 1), or even three or four cases can look faintly<br />

ridiculous. But is it?<br />

6.2 The Function <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Single Case Within a Quantitative<br />

<strong>Research</strong> Tradition<br />

It is worth noting, first <strong>of</strong> all, that <strong>the</strong> single case (<strong>and</strong> I shall use this also to include<br />

a small number <strong>of</strong> cases such that would not normally be thought to be statistically<br />

significant) can play an important <strong>and</strong> indeed devastating role even within <strong>the</strong><br />

broadly quantitative research tradition. I have thus far accepted without comment<br />

<strong>the</strong> supposition which underpins <strong>the</strong> logic <strong>of</strong> a large part <strong>of</strong> empirical research in<br />

education as elsewhere, that one can draw inferences from <strong>the</strong> an appropriately<br />

constructed sample which has been <strong>the</strong> object <strong>of</strong> study to <strong>the</strong> wider population<br />

from which it is selected: in o<strong>the</strong>r words, one can generalise from <strong>the</strong> particular

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!