06.11.2014 Views

learning-styles

learning-styles

learning-styles

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The most influential member of the cognitive structure<br />

group is Witkin, whose bipolar dimensions of field<br />

dependence/field independence have had considerable<br />

influence on the <strong>learning</strong> <strong>styles</strong> discipline, both<br />

in terms of the exploration of his own constructs<br />

and the reactions against it which have led to the<br />

development of other <strong>learning</strong> <strong>styles</strong> descriptors<br />

and instruments. The educational implications of field<br />

dependence/independence (FDI) have been explored<br />

mainly in the curriculum areas of second-language<br />

acquisition, mathematics, natural and social sciences<br />

(see Tinajero and Paramo 1998a for a review of this<br />

evidence), although its vogue as a purely <strong>learning</strong> <strong>styles</strong><br />

instrument has arguably passed. However, FDI remains<br />

an important concept in the understanding of individual<br />

differences in motor skills performance (Brady 1995)<br />

and musical discrimination (Ellis 1996).<br />

Three tests are used to study FD and FI: the Rod<br />

and Frame Test, the Body Adjustment Test and the<br />

Group Embedded Figures Test. The Rod and Frame Test<br />

involves sitting the participant in a dark room. The<br />

participant can see a luminous rod in a luminous frame.<br />

The frame is tilted and the participant is asked to make<br />

the rod vertical. Some participants move the rod so that<br />

it is in alignment with the tilted frame; others succeed<br />

in making the rod vertical. The former participants take<br />

their cues from the environment (the surrounding field)<br />

and are described as ‘field dependent’; the latter<br />

are uninfluenced by the surrounding field (the frame)<br />

and are described as ‘field independent’.<br />

The Body Adjustment Test is similar to the Rod and<br />

Frame Test in that it also involves space orientation.<br />

The participant is seated in a tilted room and asked<br />

to sit upright. Again, field-dependent participants<br />

sit in alignment with the room, while field-independent<br />

participants sit upright, independent of the angle of the<br />

room. The Group Embedded Figures Test is a paper<br />

and pencil test. The participant is shown a geometric<br />

shape and is then shown a complex shape which<br />

contains the original shape ‘hidden’ somewhere.<br />

The field-independent person can quickly find the<br />

original shape because they are not influenced by<br />

the surrounding shapes; the opposite is true of the<br />

field-dependent person. The authors claim that results<br />

from the three tests are highly correlated with each<br />

other (Witkin and Goodenough 1981).<br />

Davies (1993, 223) summarises the claims made<br />

by the authors for field dependence/independence:<br />

‘According to Witkin and Goodenough (1981),<br />

field independents are better than field dependents<br />

at tasks requiring the breaking up of an organised<br />

stimulus context into individual elements and/or<br />

the re-arranging of the individual elements to form<br />

a different organisation.’<br />

Measurement of the instruments<br />

Overall, there are two key issues in relation to<br />

the cognitive structure <strong>learning</strong> <strong>styles</strong>: the conflation<br />

of style with ability and the validity of the bipolar<br />

structure of many of the measures.<br />

Style and ability<br />

While he reports that measures of cognitive style<br />

appear to have test–retest reliability, Messick<br />

(1984, 59) considers that there is an ‘unresolved<br />

question … the extent to which the empirical<br />

consistencies attributed to cognitive <strong>styles</strong> are instead<br />

a function of intellective abilities’, since cognitive <strong>styles</strong><br />

are assessed with what he calls ‘ability-like measures’.<br />

In particular, he argues (1984, 63) that measurements<br />

of field independence and field dependence are too<br />

dependent on ability: ‘by linking global style to low<br />

analytical performance, field dependence is essentially<br />

measured by default.’<br />

That this weakness of the cognitive structure family<br />

appears to be particularly true of Witkin is borne<br />

out by empirical studies: ‘the embarrassing truth<br />

of the matter is that various investigators have found<br />

significant relations between the Witkin indexes,<br />

on the one hand, and measures of verbal, mathematical<br />

and spatial skills, on the other.’ (Kogan 1973, 166).<br />

Indeed, Federico and Landis, in their analysis of field<br />

dependence, category width and 22 other measures<br />

of cognitive characteristics, found (1984, 152) that<br />

‘all cognitive <strong>styles</strong> except reflection-impulsivity<br />

are significantly related to ability and/or aptitudes.<br />

Field independence has more (ie 10) significant<br />

correlations [ranging from 0.15 to 0.34] with abilities<br />

and aptitudes than any other style’. Huang and Chao<br />

(2000) found that in a small study (n=60, mean age 17),<br />

students with <strong>learning</strong> disabilities were more likely<br />

to be field dependent than a matched group of ‘average’<br />

students. Indeed, the construction of field dependence<br />

as a disability in itself is highlighted by Tinajero et al.<br />

(1993) who report on studies from the field of<br />

neuropsychology which attempt to link field dependence<br />

with cerebral injury, though the question as to which<br />

hemisphere is injured is an unresolved one. The<br />

theorists in the cognitive structure family take great<br />

pains to differentiate between ability and style –<br />

‘Abilities concern level of skill – the more and less<br />

of performance – whereas cognitive <strong>styles</strong> give<br />

greater weight to the manner and form of cognition’<br />

(Kogan 1973, 244; original emphasis) – but we are<br />

forced to conclude that if the measures used to assess<br />

style are too closely linked to ability tasks, then we<br />

may have what Henry Fielding in Tom Jones memorably<br />

describes as ‘a distinction without a difference’.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!