06.11.2014 Views

learning-styles

learning-styles

learning-styles

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Implications for pedagogy<br />

Riding (2002) claims that his model has important<br />

implications for many aspects of human behaviour.<br />

He believes that for less able learners, it is important<br />

to achieve a match between cognitive style, the<br />

way in which resources are structured and the teaching<br />

approach. At the same time, he acknowledges that<br />

many variables (especially working memory) interact<br />

with style to determine performance. He and his<br />

students and colleagues have carried out a large<br />

number of correlational and predictive studies focusing<br />

on <strong>learning</strong> outcomes, but it would be unwise to<br />

accept unreplicated findings in view of the problems<br />

of reliability indicated above. An instrument which is<br />

so inadequate in terms of test–retest reliability cannot<br />

be said to provide robust evidence for adopting<br />

particular strategies in post-16 <strong>learning</strong> and teaching.<br />

This point certainly holds for the CSA’s highly unreliable<br />

verbal-imager measure, but it is possible that<br />

meaningful group differences may exist in relation to<br />

the holist-analytic measure, even though its reliability<br />

is at best modest.<br />

Perhaps the most convincing study of the pedagogical<br />

implications of CSA scores in the post-16 sector is<br />

the one carried out by Sadler-Smith and Riding (1999)<br />

with 240 business studies students. Here it was found<br />

that holists favoured collaborative <strong>learning</strong> and the use<br />

of non-print materials such as overhead transparencies<br />

(OHTs), slides and videos. However, it is a step too<br />

far to move from this finding to the recommendation<br />

that students should be given what they prefer.<br />

Indeed, in a study of 112 GCSE Design and Technology<br />

students in eight schools, Atkinson (1998) found that<br />

holistic students who were taught by teachers using<br />

a collaborative approach obtained poorer grades than<br />

any other group.<br />

A small-scale study of some interest is that by<br />

Littlemore (2001), who found a significant difference<br />

between 28 holistic and 20 analytic language students.<br />

The holists tended to make greater use of analogy<br />

when unable to find the correct word when naming<br />

pictures in a second language, whereas the analysts<br />

more often used analytic strategies, such as naming<br />

parts, uses or the functions of the objects. However,<br />

the differences were not large, and as all students<br />

made use of both types of strategy, there do not seem<br />

to be any instructional implications.<br />

Riding et al. (2003, 167) acknowledge that in the<br />

past, ‘studies of style effects have often not shown<br />

clear results or have shown relatively little effect’.<br />

They suggest that this may be because interactions<br />

between individual difference variables have not<br />

been widely studied. They report on interactions<br />

between cognitive style and working memory in 206<br />

13 year olds, finding four significant effects out<br />

of 11 in the case of the holist-analytic dimension.<br />

Teacher ratings of <strong>learning</strong> behaviour and subject<br />

performance tended to be low for analytics who were<br />

below average on a working memory test, but high<br />

for analytics with above-average working memory<br />

scores. For holists, working memory was less clearly<br />

related to teacher ratings, except in mathematics.<br />

There was no convincing evidence of a similar<br />

interaction effect for the verbaliser-visualiser dimension,<br />

with only one significant result out of 11 ANOVA<br />

(analysis of variance) analyses. This study needs<br />

replication, preferably with more reliable measures<br />

of cognitive style and using a test of working memory<br />

of known reliability and validity.<br />

Positive evidence supporting the ‘matching’<br />

hypothesis as applied to the global-analytic dimension<br />

in a computer-based <strong>learning</strong> environment comes<br />

from two small-scale studies by Ford (1995) and Ford<br />

and Chen (2001). These made use of two very carefully<br />

designed ways of teaching a classification task and<br />

HTML programming, each believed to suit different ways<br />

of <strong>learning</strong>. In the second experiment, it was found that,<br />

as predicted, global learners did significantly better<br />

with ‘breadth first’ and analytic learners did best<br />

with ‘depth first’ instruction. The effect sizes in these<br />

two experiments were large, and together, the findings<br />

should be taken seriously, despite the relatively small<br />

sample sizes (34 and 57 respectively).<br />

With the exception of this last finding by independent<br />

researchers, there is a dearth of well-grounded<br />

empirical evidence to support the extensive range<br />

of pedagogical recommendations made by Riding<br />

(2002). The same is true of the set of profiles<br />

for each cognitive style which Riding (1994) has<br />

offered. These are set out in terms of:<br />

social attitude<br />

response to people<br />

decision making<br />

consistency and reliability<br />

managing and being managed<br />

<strong>learning</strong> and communication<br />

team roles<br />

response to difficulties.<br />

The research basis for these profiles is not explained,<br />

but some relevant correlational studies are summarised<br />

by Riding and Rayner (1998). However, in the case<br />

of team roles, the evidence is very slight, being based<br />

on an unpublished study involving only 10 managers.<br />

Despite these empirical drawbacks, it is possible<br />

to argue that Riding’s model, rather than the CSA,<br />

may have important implications for teaching. Although<br />

not proven by research, it is plausible that teaching<br />

which is biased towards any one of the extreme poles<br />

of the model would disadvantage some learners.<br />

If this is so, the implication is that teachers should<br />

deal both with generalities and particulars; structure<br />

material so that part-whole relationships are clear;<br />

make demands on both deductive and inductive<br />

reasoning; and make use of both visual and verbal<br />

forms of expression.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!