06.11.2014 Views

learning-styles

learning-styles

learning-styles

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Section 5<br />

Stable personality type<br />

LSRC reference<br />

page 46/47<br />

Introduction<br />

The instruments and models grouped in this family<br />

have a common focus upon <strong>learning</strong> style as one part<br />

of the observable expression of a relatively stable<br />

personality type, a theory primarily influenced by the<br />

work of Jung (1968). The most prominent theorists who<br />

operate ‘at the interface of intelligence and personality’<br />

(Grigorenko and Sternberg 1995) are Myers-Briggs<br />

(Myers and McCaulley 1985) and Jackson (2002),<br />

although they share certain key characteristics with<br />

measures developed by Bates and Keirsey (1978),<br />

Harrison and Bramson, (1982, 1988) and Miller (1991).<br />

While debates continue within psychology about<br />

the appropriate descriptors for personality traits<br />

and, indeed, how many factors underpin individual<br />

differences (see eg Furnham 1995; Jackson et al.<br />

2000), the theorists in this family are concerned<br />

with constructing instruments which embed <strong>learning</strong><br />

<strong>styles</strong> within an understanding of the personality traits<br />

that shape all aspects of an individual’s interaction<br />

with the world.<br />

The descriptors of personality are, in taxonomic<br />

terms, polythetic – that is, grouping together observed<br />

phenomena with shared features, but not excluding<br />

from groups phenomena which share some, but not all,<br />

of the relevant features (Eysenck 1997). This approach<br />

is both a strength, since it allows for reasonable<br />

variation, and a weakness, since ‘numerical solutions<br />

are essentially indeterminate in the absence of causal<br />

relations’ (Eysenck 1997, 23). Eysenck makes the<br />

argument for a distinction between the reliability<br />

of personality factors, such as those in the ‘big five’<br />

(see Section 5.1 below), which is relatively consistent<br />

and their validity, which is dependent upon a theoretical<br />

construction which allows for the causal nature<br />

of personality factors to be experimentally tested.<br />

An alternative approach – to explore genetic markers<br />

for specific, observable personality traits – has proved,<br />

as yet, elusive (Stevenson 1997) and it is therefore<br />

more difficult to trace the heritability of personality<br />

compared, for example, to measures of IQ, though<br />

there are some indications that strong traits towards<br />

extraversion overcome environmental effects in<br />

adoption and twin studies (Loehlin 1992).<br />

5.1<br />

The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) ®8<br />

Introduction<br />

The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) was designed<br />

by Katherine Cook Briggs and her daughter Isabel<br />

Briggs Myers. They began to develop their instrument<br />

in the early 1940s with the avowed aim of making<br />

Jung’s theory of human personality understandable<br />

and useful in everyday life: ‘Jung saw his theory as<br />

an aid to self-understanding, but the application of the<br />

theory (like the theory itself) extends beyond the point<br />

where Jung was content to stop.’ (Myers, quoted by<br />

Mosley 2001, 8). This resulted in the publication of the<br />

first MBTI manual in 1962, the subsequent versions<br />

of which (Myers and McCaulley 1985, 1998) are most<br />

frequently referred to in studies drawn on for this review.<br />

The MBTI focuses more upon the description of normally<br />

observed types, rather than idealised theoretical<br />

types which, as Jung himself argued, would rarely be<br />

met in everyday life (Jung, quoted by Mosley 2001, 3).<br />

In terms of academic heritage, the MBTI has often<br />

been strongly linked to personality instruments<br />

using the ‘big five’ personality factors (extraversion,<br />

openness, agreeableness, conscientiousness and<br />

neuroticism – the last of which is not included in<br />

the MBTI), exemplified by the most popular instrument<br />

in personality testing in the UK and the US, the<br />

NEO-Personality Inventory (McCrae and Costa 1987).<br />

However, the MBTI differs strongly from the NEO-PI<br />

and other instruments in that it is, according to<br />

Quenck (2003):<br />

a theory-based instrument grounded in Jung’s typology<br />

rather than an empirically derived trait instrument …<br />

neuroticism is not part of the MBTI because Jung did<br />

not include such a dimension in his typology, which was<br />

meant to reflect normal, non-pathological personality<br />

differences. It is for that reason that the opposite<br />

poles of each of the dichotomies are conceptualized<br />

as qualitatively distinct and opposite to each other,<br />

with each pole defined as legitimate in its own right.<br />

One pole is never described as indicating a ‘deficit’<br />

in the opposite pole, or [as being] more valued than<br />

the other pole, as is the case in the NEO-PI and other<br />

trait conceptions of personality.<br />

8<br />

Myers-Briggs Type Indicator and MBTI are registered trademarks of CPP Inc,<br />

Palo Alto, California.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!