06.11.2014 Views

learning-styles

learning-styles

learning-styles

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

LSRC reference Section 4<br />

page 44/45<br />

Table 10<br />

Riding’s Cognitive Styles<br />

Analysis (CSA)<br />

General<br />

Strengths<br />

Learning strategies may be learned<br />

and improved.<br />

Weaknesses<br />

‘Default’ <strong>learning</strong> <strong>styles</strong> are assumed<br />

to be fixed.<br />

Design of the model<br />

Two dimensions which are independent<br />

of intelligence: holist-analytic<br />

(ways of organising information) and<br />

verbaliser-imager (ways of representing<br />

information).<br />

Two very specific tasks bear the weight<br />

of broad and loosely defined constructs.<br />

Deals with cognitive, not affective<br />

or conative aspects of thinking<br />

and <strong>learning</strong>.<br />

Reliability<br />

No evidence provided by the author.<br />

Others have shown that internal<br />

consistency and test–retest reliability<br />

is very poor, especially for the<br />

verbaliser-imager ratio score.<br />

Validity<br />

Both dimensions have reasonable<br />

face validity.<br />

The holist-analytic measure may<br />

be useful for assessing group rather<br />

than individual differences.<br />

Performance is sampled over a very<br />

limited range of task difficulty.<br />

As the reliability of the CSA is so<br />

poor, studies of validity should<br />

not be accepted unless they have<br />

been replicated.<br />

Implications<br />

for pedagogy<br />

There is evidence of links<br />

between cognitive <strong>styles</strong> and<br />

instructional preferences.<br />

There is evidence that in computerised<br />

instruction, ‘holist’ learners do<br />

better with ‘breadth first’ and ‘analytic’<br />

learners with ‘depth first’.<br />

Riding claims that teachers need to<br />

take account of individual differences<br />

in working memory as well as style.<br />

Most teachers use a variety<br />

of instructional approaches<br />

anyway (eg verbal and visual).<br />

A large number of recommendations<br />

are made without adequate<br />

empirical evidence.<br />

Evidence of<br />

pedagogical impact<br />

Inconclusive.<br />

Overall assessment<br />

Key source<br />

The simplicity and potential value of Riding’s model are not well served by an<br />

unreliable instrument, the CSA.<br />

Riding and Rayner 1998<br />

Empirical evidence of pedagogical impact<br />

Although there are many published studies in which<br />

significant differences in <strong>learning</strong> outcomes have<br />

been found between groups with different CSA scores,<br />

we do not consider that these studies provide more<br />

than interesting suggestions for pedagogical practice.<br />

We are not aware of any lasting changes in instructional<br />

practice which have been brought about as a result<br />

of using the CSA on a regular basis.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!