06.11.2014 Views

learning-styles

learning-styles

learning-styles

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The theory is an evolving one and Apter (2001, 307)<br />

acknowledges the need for ‘a systematic developmental<br />

underpinning for the theory’ as well as the ‘need to<br />

develop specific techniques that would allow people<br />

to come more in control of their own reversal processes’<br />

(2001, 306). This is a difficult area, since Apter has<br />

posited an unconscious biological basis for reversal<br />

without fully accounting for its adaptive value. There<br />

is, nonetheless, an impressive amount of empirical<br />

evidence which supports reversal theory. Apter and<br />

Heskin (2001) have summarised the research evidence<br />

which supports the basic propositions of the theory,<br />

including some studies in which reversal was monitored<br />

during educationally relevant activities such as studying<br />

statistics and reading.<br />

While Apter does not claim that his four domains are<br />

the only way of conceptualising psychological needs,<br />

he does (2001, 39) claim exhaustiveness in the sense<br />

that for a given pair of motivational states, ‘one or the<br />

other will be active during the whole of waking life’.<br />

He allows that a pair of states may be more or less<br />

central or peripheral in awareness, but not that<br />

both may disappear altogether from consciousness.<br />

However, it is not clear whether this is a logical or<br />

empirical claim, and if the latter, whether it is falsifiable.<br />

Apter does not seem to allow for the simultaneous<br />

activation of pairs of states such as goal-oriented<br />

(telic) and activity-oriented (paratelic). Yet if<br />

simultaneous activation does not occur, it is difficult<br />

to explain behaviour where both are required, such<br />

as the performance of musicians and stand-up comics,<br />

where the experience of flow is at once enjoyment<br />

and achievement.<br />

Apter’s treatment of arousal-avoidance and<br />

arousal-seeking is not fully consistent, since these<br />

are assimilated within the telic-paratelic dimension<br />

in much of his writing, but treated as a separate dimensi<br />

on in the MSP. The MSP approach is more convincing,<br />

since while peace and quiet may generally help people<br />

to focus on goal achievement, this is not always so.<br />

Reversal theory is based on clear definitions and has<br />

a clear structure, despite the use of invented terms<br />

to refer to the poles of two dimensions (‘telic’ and<br />

‘paratelic’ in the case of the means-end dimension<br />

and ‘autic’ and ‘alloic’ as applied to relationships).<br />

While some features of the theory can be questioned,<br />

Apter (2001) has set it out in a highly coherent form,<br />

with four basic assumptions and 10 basic propositions.<br />

Although it is a theory of personality rather than<br />

of <strong>learning</strong>, reversal theory does provide a conceptual<br />

framework for asking questions in a systematic way<br />

about approaches to <strong>learning</strong>, especially about<br />

motivation, feelings about <strong>learning</strong> and personal style.<br />

Its dimensions are not new, but the concept of reversal<br />

is refreshingly novel and provides a real challenge<br />

to theorists who seek to pigeonhole individuals in terms<br />

of fixed characteristics.<br />

It is helpful to consider reversal theory in the context<br />

of other theories and models of thinking, <strong>learning</strong><br />

and personal style. Apter’s telic-paratelic dimension<br />

is conceptually linked with extrinsic versus intrinsic<br />

motivation and with convergent versus divergent<br />

thinking. A telic orientation may also be what motivates<br />

some learners to approach study with the aim of gaining<br />

high examination marks, while some students who<br />

do not take their studies seriously may have a paratelic<br />

orientation. Deep absorption in studying a subject<br />

can be an end in itself or be motivated by a serious<br />

academic ambition, while ‘surface’ learners may<br />

become more interested if teachers find ways of making<br />

<strong>learning</strong> more enjoyable. There is a family resemblance<br />

between Apter’s conformist-negativistic dimension,<br />

Sternberg’s (1998) hierarchic and anarchic thinking<br />

<strong>styles</strong> and Kirton’s distinction (1989) between adaptors<br />

and innovators. Apter’s concept of autic mastery<br />

reflects values of individualism and competitiveness,<br />

while alloic sympathy reflects values of social belonging<br />

and cooperation.<br />

Most importantly, the key concept of reversal has major<br />

implications for how we think about <strong>learning</strong> <strong>styles</strong>.<br />

It leads us to expect reversals between <strong>learning</strong> <strong>styles</strong><br />

as well as some degree of individual consistency<br />

over time, and it strongly suggests that productive<br />

<strong>learning</strong> <strong>styles</strong> can be fostered by creating <strong>learning</strong><br />

environments though which important values are<br />

conveyed and in which reversals through boredom<br />

and satiation are less likely to occur.<br />

Evaluation of the MSP and of related instruments<br />

The MSP items are written in simple language, with<br />

a readability level of about 9 years. Most are clearly<br />

expressed, but some (especially those beginning<br />

with ‘I like…’) can be read in more than one way.<br />

For example, I may respond that I always ‘like to be<br />

liked’, meaning that being liked is a common experience<br />

for me; or I may, by the same response, mean that<br />

I always like the experience of being liked, even though<br />

I do not have it very often.<br />

The MSP is fairly robust in psychometric terms,<br />

with internal consistency of the 14 sub-scales in the<br />

range 0.64 to 0.89 for the UK version and test–retest<br />

correlations in the range 0.73 to 0.92 over a 12-week<br />

period (Apter, Mallows and Williams 1998). The<br />

most stable sub-scales were those for other-oriented<br />

affection, optimism, excitement and fun.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!