31.08.2013 Views

waders and their estuarine food supplies - Vlaams Instituut voor de ...

waders and their estuarine food supplies - Vlaams Instituut voor de ...

waders and their estuarine food supplies - Vlaams Instituut voor de ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

100 200 300<br />

prey <strong>de</strong>nsity (n-m-2)<br />

Fig. I. Relationship between the time spent probing by the Oystercatcher<br />

(as a percentage ol available foraging time minus total h<strong>and</strong>ling<br />

lime) ami the <strong>de</strong>nsit) I D)oi Scrobicularia plana Means ±SE<br />

(sample sizes indicated) arc shown for all offered <strong>de</strong>nsities ir»l.<br />

During three successive sessions al l>= 2 in ' the bird found nn prey<br />

al all <strong>and</strong> the amount of probim; simngly <strong>de</strong>creased. Mean percentage<br />

probing lime ± SE tO) per 5 min obscivaiion nine is given lor<br />

(a) lirsi session (40 mini. Ibi second session after40 nun 130 mini:<br />

le) third session after 19 h (20 min).<br />

lion completely before any session, <strong>and</strong> in most sessions<br />

the bird was only allowed to take a few prey.<br />

We controlled hunger level by allowing the bird the<br />

same amounl of <strong>food</strong> each day; viz. 35 g (AFDW): this<br />

is the mean <strong>food</strong> requirement of an Oystetcaichei in<br />

captivity withabodv weight of 460 g (Hulscher 1974).<br />

The extra <strong>food</strong> nee<strong>de</strong>d to make up this amount was<br />

given lo the bird al ihe end of the daily experimental<br />

sessions in the form of opened Mussels Myiilus editlis<br />

L. <strong>and</strong> Scrobicularia in ihe non-experimental cage.<br />

Any remaining fcxxl was taken away every evening at<br />

23.30 h. The experiments started each morning at<br />

(WOO h. so the bird was <strong>de</strong>prived of fcxxl for 9.5 h.<br />

I his is about 3 I) longer than usually occurs in ihe field.<br />

so our captive bird was always motivated to feed<br />

The experimental sessions<br />

The following were measured during a session:<br />

(I) Available foraging time (duration of a session).<br />

The Oystercatcher always entered the experimental<br />

cage immediately after the passage was opened. After<br />

OPTIMAL FORAGING AND THE FUNCTIONAL RESPONSE<br />

141<br />

it had eaten the allowed number of prey, the passage<br />

was opened again <strong>and</strong> the bird driven out.<br />

(2) Searching lime. Since we assumed searching<br />

was bj touch, only the period in which the bill was beneath<br />

the mud surface <strong>and</strong> no h<strong>and</strong>ling could be observed<br />

was scored as searching lime. This probing<br />

time' amounted to 30-40% of the total lime minus h<strong>and</strong>ling<br />

time (Fig. 1). but was lower at the lowest prey<br />

<strong>de</strong>nsity offered (2 prey per nr). In this case ihe bird did<br />

not find a prey after probing for 15 min. so this <strong>de</strong>nsit]<br />

was not used in the analysts<br />

(3) Depth of located prey. We noted ihe coordinates<br />

of prey that were found so dial after the experiment we<br />

could <strong>de</strong>termine its <strong>de</strong>pth,<br />

14i H<strong>and</strong>ling time. As with Oystercatchers feeding<br />

on Macoma balihica (Hulscher 1982) our bird h<strong>and</strong>led<br />

prey in two ways: ui) opening the shell in situ, or (b)<br />

lifting the shell from the mud before opening. We always<br />

noted which method was used <strong>and</strong> measured total<br />

h<strong>and</strong>ling time by stopwatch. We also measured the<br />

lifting lime (time nee<strong>de</strong>d to pull the shell to the surface<br />

<strong>and</strong> to put it down); cutting lime (time nee<strong>de</strong>d to remove<br />

the flesh from the shell), <strong>and</strong> eating time (time<br />

nee<strong>de</strong>d to wash <strong>and</strong> swallow the lleshi.<br />

(5) Number of prey taken. The number of prey the<br />

bird was allowed to take in one session <strong>de</strong>pen<strong>de</strong>d on<br />

prey <strong>de</strong>nsity: one prey at D = 2 or 6 nr 2 ; two prey<br />

at Da 12 or 24 nr 2 : three prey at D = 44 nr 2 ; five prey<br />

at D=88m" a <strong>and</strong> ten prey (3-7%) from <strong>de</strong>nsity 144<br />

nr 2 onwards. Because of the very low prey <strong>de</strong>pletion,<br />

there was no need to correct for <strong>de</strong>creasing prey <strong>de</strong>nsity<br />

during the course of a session (Rogers 1972).<br />

However, the fact that the number of prey the bird was<br />

allowed to take, increased with prey <strong>de</strong>nsity, might<br />

have affected the <strong>de</strong>cision making process of the Oystercatcher.<br />

As shown by Lucas t 1983) the bird might<br />

lower its selection criterion when the remaining time<br />

available for feeding <strong>de</strong>creases Since not time itself<br />

was limited, but the number of prey allowed, time constraints<br />

are unlikely to have affected ihe optimal diet<br />

choice in these experiments.<br />

The analysis would have been complicated if the<br />

bird had increased iis h<strong>and</strong>ling of searching lime during<br />

long sessions because of <strong>de</strong>creasing motivation<br />

(Holling 1966|. However, this appeared not to happen:<br />

in 21 of the 24 sessions in which the bird tcxik 5 or<br />

more prey, there were no significant trends in ihe han-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!