31.08.2013 Views

waders and their estuarine food supplies - Vlaams Instituut voor de ...

waders and their estuarine food supplies - Vlaams Instituut voor de ...

waders and their estuarine food supplies - Vlaams Instituut voor de ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>and</strong> maiiv are too thick-shelled to be opened (Kg, 2).<br />

like slabbing Oystercatchers. hammering birds reject<br />

Mussels below 20 to 25 mm long. bin. in contrast to<br />

slabbers. Mussels larger than 60 mm are laken only infrequently.<br />

When a correction is ma<strong>de</strong> for the prey<br />

fraction thai is unavailable due to shell thickness <strong>and</strong><br />

barnacle cover (Fig. 2), the proportion or each large<br />

prey class taken roughly coinci<strong>de</strong>s with <strong>their</strong> available<br />

<strong>de</strong>nsity (Meire & Ervynck 1986). or explains al least a<br />

part of the <strong>de</strong>viation between observed size selection<br />

<strong>and</strong> frequency distributioii of si/e classes on offer<br />

(CayfordcS Goss-Custard 1990).<br />

Size selection <strong>and</strong> optimal foraging<br />

Predicted 'active si/e selection'<br />

The r<strong>and</strong>om touch mo<strong>de</strong>l tests the assumption that<br />

birds lake prey al r<strong>and</strong>om. In fact, as the results in the<br />

previous section show, the observed prev selection often<br />

<strong>de</strong>viates from the predictions of the mo<strong>de</strong>l. Oystercatchers<br />

apparently prefer some si/e class to others.<br />

Why? Prev size selection in Oystercatcher is analysed<br />

here within the framework of optimal foraging theory<br />

(Enilen 1966. Mac Arthur & Pianka 1966. Krebs &<br />

Kacelnik 1991). The basic assumption is thai predators<br />

are able to rank prey according to <strong>their</strong> profitability,<br />

<strong>de</strong>fined as ihe intake rate while prey are being h<strong>and</strong>led.<br />

They are predicted to reject prey for which the profitability<br />

is below the current average intake rate over<br />

both h<strong>and</strong>ling <strong>and</strong> searching combined. The <strong>de</strong>cision<br />

rule governing the rejection threshold is therefore<br />

based on the relative profitability of h<strong>and</strong>ling compared<br />

with continuing to search, i.e. whether the bird<br />

can achieve a higher net intake rate by continuing to<br />

search for more profitable prey than it could achieve<br />

by h<strong>and</strong>ling a less profitable, although more frequently<br />

encountered, prey. This leads lo the prediction thai.<br />

when the profitability of the prey remains the same, the<br />

rejection threshold should increase as ihe intake rate<br />

increases. The rejection threshold should also increase<br />

when the iniake rale remains the same but the profiiabiliiv<br />

of all prey types <strong>de</strong>creases: lor instance, because<br />

the prey are lean. The rejection threshold should<br />

therefore be flexible within clearly <strong>de</strong>fined limits, as illustrated<br />

in Fig. 8.<br />

PREY SIZE SELECTION AND INTAKE RATE<br />

162<br />

10 20 30 40<br />

length of prey (mm)<br />

intake rate<br />

lotworwiil cangal<br />

Fig. 8. The optimal pre) size selection mo<strong>de</strong>l. The two slopes <strong>de</strong>limit<br />

the seasonal variation in profitability (mg a ' h<strong>and</strong>ling) as a<br />

Innclionol prev si/e I'rev lor which ihe prolil.ihililv is below Ihe illlake<br />

rale line s' feeding, i.e. during both searching <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>ling)<br />

should be rejected. Which pre) shook) he rejected thus <strong>de</strong>pends on<br />

the level oi the intake raie .is well a-on ihe length profitability slope.<br />

The .lark sha<strong>de</strong>d Held shows the expected range within which the<br />

lower acceptance threshold should he lound when Ihe intake rale<br />

varies between I <strong>and</strong> 4 mg s ' feeding.<br />

Small prey are less profitable<br />

Even after a correction has been ma<strong>de</strong> for the small<br />

'effective touch area" oi small prey,Oystercatchers appear<br />

lo lake fewer oi them than would be expected on<br />

Ihe basis of the frequency with which they are encountered<br />

(Figs. 2 to 7). The birds always completely reject<br />

prey less than about 10 mm long in Macoma <strong>and</strong><br />

Cerasto<strong>de</strong>rma <strong>and</strong> about 15 to 20 mm long in Scrobicularia.<br />

Mya <strong>and</strong> Myiilus. There might be a very simple<br />

explanation for this. Oystercatchers are specialized<br />

to open hard-shelled prey before they eat the llesh. in<br />

contrast lo Knot Calidris canutus or Bar-tailed Godwits<br />

limosa lapponica which swallow ihe prey whole<br />

<strong>and</strong> crush them in the stomach. There must be a size below<br />

which Oystercatchers are hardly able to separate<br />

the flesh from the shell. Whether or not this is close to<br />

ihe observed rejection threshold has still to be tested.<br />

On the other h<strong>and</strong>, several papers have used the<br />

optimality approach to explain the size rejection<br />

threshold of Oystercatchers. The prediction is that, below<br />

a certain size threshold, prev are simply noi worth<br />

taking since <strong>their</strong> energv value is too low given the<br />

time required to h<strong>and</strong>le them: i.e. to open them <strong>and</strong> eat<br />

50

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!