17.01.2013 Views

Cash or Card: Consumer Perceptions of Payment Modes - Scholarly ...

Cash or Card: Consumer Perceptions of Payment Modes - Scholarly ...

Cash or Card: Consumer Perceptions of Payment Modes - Scholarly ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

and Belmont (1996) found that restaurant diners gave higher tips on tip trays containing<br />

credit card symbols when compared to diners who received blank tip trays. McCall,<br />

Trombetta, and Gipe (2004) found that credit card symbols had a similar effect on estimated<br />

tip sizes in a lab<strong>or</strong>at<strong>or</strong>y setting. A New Zealand study by Lie, Hunt, Peters, Velin and Harper<br />

(2010) found that negative associations with credit cards limit the use <strong>of</strong> such cards and limit<br />

spending when used. Their study showed that participants were less likely to spend in the<br />

presence <strong>of</strong> credit card logos. So the effects <strong>of</strong> the credit card logo are not clear. This may be<br />

due to the research context and design <strong>or</strong> there may be cultural fact<strong>or</strong>s at play.<br />

<strong>Payment</strong> mode effects- credit cards and cash<br />

Studies since 2000 are primarily lab<strong>or</strong>at<strong>or</strong>y experiments. Prelec and Simester (2001)<br />

compared behaviour across two payment modes. In their first study they auctioned tickets<br />

(via a sealed-bid system) f<strong>or</strong> a sp<strong>or</strong>ting event and allowed the use <strong>of</strong> cash <strong>or</strong> credit card<br />

payment modes. The average price paid by the group who were expecting to pay by credit<br />

card was significantly higher than the average price paid by the group who were expecting to<br />

pay cash. In the second study they randomly assigned participants to a credit <strong>or</strong> cash payment<br />

mode and assessed their willingness to pay f<strong>or</strong> a $175 gift card to a local restaurant. They did<br />

not find a difference. They argue that liquidity constraints cannot completely explain these<br />

results. However, those participants could choose their payment mode in the first test and not<br />

the second may be a fact<strong>or</strong>, and in the second test the students may not have had an interest in<br />

n<strong>or</strong> valued the restaurant voucher.<br />

Soman (2001) examined the effect <strong>of</strong> past usage <strong>of</strong> payment mechanisms on future spending<br />

behaviour by creating differences in the retrospective evaluation <strong>of</strong> past payments. He found<br />

that consumers who n<strong>or</strong>mally use credit cards vs. cheque to pay f<strong>or</strong> transactions in the test<br />

situation were m<strong>or</strong>e likely to purchase an additional discretionary product. Raghubir and<br />

Srivastava (2008) conducted four separate experiments in a single study to examine the<br />

relationship between spending behaviour and the mode <strong>of</strong> payment. The first study replicated<br />

the Feinberg (1986) study and found similar results - namely that individuals are willing to<br />

spend m<strong>or</strong>e when exposed to a credit card logo. The second study examined willingness to<br />

spend via cash and credit card by manipulating the amount <strong>of</strong> the transaction. First they<br />

asked participants to estimate the cost <strong>of</strong> individual items to ascertain if it was considered to<br />

be a small payment (cost), and once agreement was reached, their willingness to use cash <strong>or</strong><br />

their credit card to pay f<strong>or</strong> each item was determined. Then the items were grouped together<br />

18

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!