01.02.2015 Views

69249454-chandler-semiotics

69249454-chandler-semiotics

69249454-chandler-semiotics

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

CODES 153<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8222<br />

9<br />

10<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

20<br />

1222<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9<br />

30<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7222<br />

SOCIAL CODES<br />

Constructionist theorists argue that linguistic codes play a key role<br />

in the construction and maintenance of social realities. We learn not<br />

the world but the codes into which it has been structured. The<br />

Whorfian hypothesis, or Sapir–Whorf theory, is named after the<br />

American linguists Edward Sapir and Benjamin Lee Whorf. In its<br />

most extreme version the Sapir–Whorf hypothesis can be described<br />

as relating two associated principles: linguistic determinism and<br />

linguistic relativism. Applying these two principles, the Whorfian<br />

thesis is that people who speak languages with very different phonological,<br />

grammatical and semantic distinctions perceive and think<br />

about the world quite differently, their worldviews being shaped or<br />

determined by their language (Sapir 1958, 69; Whorf 1956, 213–14).<br />

The extreme determinist form of the Sapir–Whorf hypothesis is<br />

rejected by most contemporary linguists. Critics note that we cannot<br />

make inferences about differences in worldview solely on the basis<br />

of differences in linguistic structure. While few linguists would<br />

accept the Whorfian hypothesis in its ‘strong’, extreme or deterministic<br />

form, many now accept a ‘weak’, more moderate, or limited<br />

Whorfianism, namely that the ways in which we see the world may<br />

be influenced by the kind of language we use.<br />

Within a culture, social differentiation is overdetermined by a<br />

multitude of social codes. We communicate our social identities<br />

through the work we do, the way we talk, the clothes we wear, our<br />

hairstyles, our eating habits, our domestic environments and possessions,<br />

our use of leisure time, our modes of travelling and so on.<br />

Language use acts as a key marker of social identity. A controversial<br />

distinction regarding British linguistic usage was introduced in<br />

the 1960s by the sociologist Basil Bernstein between so-called<br />

‘restricted code’ and ‘elaborated code’ (Bernstein 1971). Restricted<br />

code was used in informal situations and was characterized by a<br />

reliance on situational context, a lack of stylistic variety, an emphasis<br />

on the speaker’s membership of the group, simple syntax and the<br />

frequent use of gestures and tag questions (such as ‘Isn’t it’).<br />

Elaborated code was used in formal situations and was characterized<br />

by less dependence on context, wide stylistic range (including

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!