03.03.2015 Views

2000115-Strengthening-Communities-with-Neighborhood-Data

2000115-Strengthening-Communities-with-Neighborhood-Data

2000115-Strengthening-Communities-with-Neighborhood-Data

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Advances in Analytic Methods for <strong>Neighborhood</strong> <strong>Data</strong> 347<br />

ture, Galster (2002) finds consistent threshold effects on various resident<br />

behaviors when the neighborhood poverty rates exceed 15 to 20 percent.<br />

Galster et al. (2010) and Hedman and Galster (2013) find substantial negative<br />

effects on individual Stockholm residents’ incomes if they reside in<br />

neighborhoods <strong>with</strong> over 40 percent low-income neighbors.<br />

Often embedded in neighborhood effect theory is the implicit assumption<br />

that neighborhoods are static. <strong>Neighborhood</strong>s are often discussed as<br />

separate entities providing a relatively constant context that consistently<br />

affects individuals who are exposed to it for an extended period (e.g.,<br />

childhood upbringing). The neighborhood’s status in the urban hierarchy<br />

is typically regarded as fixed over time, as are the features that make up<br />

its opportunity structures. For example, an area once defined as a poverty<br />

area is implicitly assumed to remain a poverty area, <strong>with</strong> essentially<br />

unaltered or only slowly changing physical and population characteristics.<br />

Of course, neighborhoods typically are dynamic; they often change<br />

their aggregate demographic and socioeconomic profiles <strong>with</strong> frequently<br />

selective moves in and out by households. The selectivity of who stays,<br />

who moves in, and who moves out can maintain, improve, or impair a<br />

neighborhood’s status position in the urban hierarchy and its internal<br />

social dynamics. But, more fundamentally, the dynamism of the neighborhood<br />

context can directly shape the magnitude of any measured<br />

neighborhood effect by shaping the duration of exposure (Galster 2012).<br />

The temporal dimension of neighborhood effects has been made<br />

more explicit in limited empirical work, <strong>with</strong> a few studies paying attention<br />

to how variations in the timing and duration of exposure modified<br />

the observed relationship. They paint a consistent portrait, however, that<br />

neighborhood effects seem to be stronger if the exposure is cumulative,<br />

and sometimes effects appear only after a lag. Aaronson (1998) examined<br />

how neighborhood poverty rates affected teens’ school dropout rates<br />

and found that the average (cumulative) neighborhood conditions experienced<br />

between ages 10 and 18 were much stronger predictors than contemporaneous<br />

conditions. Wheaton and Clarke (2003) investigated the<br />

temporal dimension of neighborhood disadvantage effects on the mental<br />

health of young adults. They found that current neighborhood had<br />

no effect, but earlier neighborhood disadvantage experienced as a child<br />

had a lagged effect that grew stronger as cumulative exposure intensified.<br />

Turley (2003) found that white (though not black) children’s school<br />

test scores and several behavioral indicators grew more efficacious the<br />

greater the mean income of their neighborhoods. These relationships

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!