03.03.2015 Views

2000115-Strengthening-Communities-with-Neighborhood-Data

2000115-Strengthening-Communities-with-Neighborhood-Data

2000115-Strengthening-Communities-with-Neighborhood-Data

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

374 <strong>Strengthening</strong> <strong>Communities</strong> <strong>with</strong> <strong>Neighborhood</strong> <strong>Data</strong><br />

residents in both tracts, shared economic conditions in both tracts (such<br />

as a factory closing that affected both or cheaper land values in both),<br />

and interactions between these dynamics. In spatial point pattern analysis,<br />

the difference between “true and apparent contagion” (<strong>with</strong> origins<br />

in epidemiology) is analogous to that between spatial dependence and<br />

spatial heterogeneity in the analysis of spatial areas. In a process of true<br />

contagion, two people end up <strong>with</strong> a similar characteristic as a result of<br />

an interaction in which the characteristic was transmitted or shared. In<br />

a process of apparent contagion, two people have similar characteristics,<br />

but this similarity is independent of any interaction they might have.<br />

True and apparent contagions are observationally equivalent (i.e., the<br />

same observed results could be reached through different unobserved<br />

processes). Since the underlying spatial process that drives the observed<br />

spatial pattern is unobserved, true and apparent contagion cannot be<br />

distinguished in a cross-sectional context <strong>with</strong>out additional information,<br />

such as additional spatially independent observations or additional<br />

observations from different time periods.<br />

Spatial feedback effects that exist under conditions of true contagion<br />

violate the lack of interaction assumption whereby treatments for one<br />

person do not influence others’ outcomes. This logic of independence<br />

was based on randomized controlled trials designed to isolate impacts<br />

of single interventions. However, part of what makes spatially clustered<br />

problems “wickedly complex” is that they have interacting causes (Bellefontaine<br />

and Wisener 2011). Here individual-level outcomes are often<br />

dependent, as in spillover effects on a single outcome or between different<br />

outcomes (Bloom 2005). In spillovers among single outcomes,<br />

the outcome of one or more people influences that of others (e.g., in<br />

peer effects, when employment increases as a result of networking <strong>with</strong><br />

employed neighbors). Spillovers between different outcomes occur when<br />

one outcome influences a different one, as in neighborhood effects when<br />

the chance that a child does well in school would be lower in a neighborhood<br />

of concentrated poverty than in a more affluent one.<br />

At the aggregate level, the concept of spatial dependence can focus<br />

attention on potential interactions between a target area and neighboring<br />

areas, including intended and unintended consequences. For instance,<br />

one such question is whether a place-based approach has positive spatial<br />

externalities (such as economic multiplier effects in neighboring areas)<br />

or negative spatial externalities (which may occur if a crime reduction<br />

effort in one precinct ends up displacing crime to nearby precincts, 1 or if

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!