14.03.2014 Views

SLEEP 2011 Abstract Supplement

SLEEP 2011 Abstract Supplement

SLEEP 2011 Abstract Supplement

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

A. Basic Science IX. Learning, Memory and Cognition<br />

agreement with enhanced flow of sensory information from the visual<br />

cortex to more frontal cortical areas. This increased posterior-to-anterior<br />

connectivity during eyes-open no longer occurred after sleep deprivation.<br />

In the opposite direction, anterior-to-posterior, GC was not different<br />

between eyes-open and eyes-closed during waking after normal<br />

sleep or sleep deprivation.<br />

Conclusion: Sleep deprivation impairs the increased posterior-to-anterior<br />

information flow that is normally present during eyes-open wakefulness.<br />

This effect was limited to the posterior-to-anterior direction, which<br />

is involved in the transfer of sensory information to the frontal cortex.<br />

These findings suggest that SWA has a preferential directionality in its<br />

restorative action, which is necessary to maintain the next day’s efficient<br />

transmission of sensory information to higher order cortical areas.<br />

Support (If Any): Funded by the Netherlands Organization of Scientific<br />

Research (NWO) VICI Grant 453.07.001 to EvS.<br />

0224<br />

<strong>SLEEP</strong> IS MORE THAN REST<br />

Nissen C, Piosczyk H, Holz J, Feige B, Voderholzer U, Riemann D<br />

University of Freiburg Medical Center, Freiburg, Germany<br />

Introduction: Sleep has been shown to facilitate neural and behavioral<br />

plasticity compared to active wakefulness. However, conflicting hypotheses<br />

propose that sleep-specific brain activity after training fosters neural<br />

reorganization (sleep hypothesis), or alternatively, that sleep provides<br />

a window of reduced stimulus interference passively protecting novel<br />

memories (rest hypothesis).<br />

Methods: One hundret thirteen healthy subjects (aged 16 to 30 yrs.)<br />

were tested on a basic texture discrimination task in the morning and<br />

retested in the afternoon, after a 1 hour period of daytime sleep, passive<br />

waking with maximally reduced interference, or active waking. Changes<br />

in texture discrimination performance have been shown to depend on<br />

local synaptic plasticity in the primary visual cortex.<br />

Results: Active and passive wakefulness were associated with deterioration<br />

in performance, presumably due to synaptic over-potentiation<br />

across within-day sessions. In contrast, sleep not only restored performance<br />

in comparison to active waking, as has been shown previously,<br />

but also in direct comparison to passive waking. Control experiments<br />

excluded that the detrimental effects of wakefulness were due to stress<br />

or fatigue. The restoration of performance across periods of sleep correlated<br />

with electroencephalographic slow wave activity, potentially related<br />

to synaptic downscaling<br />

Conclusion: We conclude that sleep is more than a resting state of reduced<br />

stimulus interference, but actively restores performance, presumably<br />

by refining underlying synaptic plasticity.<br />

0225<br />

<strong>SLEEP</strong> PROMOTES CONSOLIDATION AND<br />

GENERALIZATION OF EXTINCTION LEARNING IN<br />

SIMULATED EXPOSURE THERAPY FOR SPIDER FEAR<br />

Pace-Schott EF 1,2 , Bennett T 1 , Verga P 1 , Hong J 1 , Spencer R 1<br />

1<br />

Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Massachusetts, Amherst,<br />

MA, USA, 2 Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA<br />

Introduction: We examined sleep’s effects in a model of exposure therapy<br />

(therapeutic extinction) for simple phobia. Given that extinction of<br />

experimental fear conditioning generalizes over sleep, we hypothesized<br />

that sleep would also allow extinction learning to generalize from an<br />

extinguished phobic object to a novel one.<br />

Methods: 32 females (age=20.1, SD=1.9) within spider-fearing ranges<br />

on Fear of Spiders (FSQ, 101.9, SD=14.6) and Spider Phobia (SPQ,<br />

23.9, SD=4.0) questionnaires, were pseudorandomly assigned to a Sleep<br />

(N=14) or Wake (N=18) group. Groups did not differ in age, FSQ,<br />

SPQ, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality or sleepiness scales. During Session 1<br />

(“Sess1”: evening Sleep, morning Wake), participants viewed 14, 60-<br />

sec videos of the same spider and wrote ratings (-10 to +10) for Disgust,<br />

Fear and Unpleasantness. Session 2 (“Sess2”) occurred 12 hours later<br />

with 6 videos of the “Old” (Sess1) spider and 6 of a “Novel” spider. A<br />

10-msec, 83dB white noise stimulus was delivered during ~75% of videos.<br />

Skin conductance responses (SCR) were continuously monitored.<br />

Four 6-video “Phases” included “Exposure_1” (Sess1, videos 1-6), “Exposure_2”<br />

(Sess1, 7-12), “Sess2_Old” (videos 1-6) and “Sess2_Novel”<br />

(7-12). Each Phase included 4 “SCR-to-noise” stimuli.<br />

Results: There was a significant Phase x Group interaction (p

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!