16.11.2012 Views

Brain–Computer Interfaces - Index of

Brain–Computer Interfaces - Index of

Brain–Computer Interfaces - Index of

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

190 F. Nijboer and U. Broermann<br />

that the chance an organism will perform a particular kind <strong>of</strong> behaviour increases<br />

when it is rewarded for it, and decreases when it is punished or not rewarded for<br />

it. A good example <strong>of</strong> operant conditioning in our own life can be observed when<br />

our email program produces its typical sound for incoming email, we rush to the<br />

inbox, because we have been rewarded with the sight <strong>of</strong> new emails in the past<br />

and we expect to be rewarded again. Our behaviour, the checking <strong>of</strong> the inbox, will<br />

vanish after we’ve heard the typical sound many times without having the reward<br />

<strong>of</strong> a new email. When behaviour vanishes, because no reward for the behaviour is<br />

given, psychologists call this extinction. The same principle can now be applied to<br />

Brain–computer interface use. When the user produces brain signals that result in<br />

a successful manipulation <strong>of</strong> the environment (reward), the likelihood that the user<br />

will produce the brain signal again in future increases.<br />

In 2006, Birbaumer [21] suggested why learning through operant conditioning<br />

might be very difficult, if not impossible, for complete locked-in patients<br />

(see also chapter “Brain–Computer Interface in Neurorehabilitation” in this book).<br />

Birbaumer remembered a series <strong>of</strong> studies on rats [40] paralyzed with curare. The<br />

rats were artificially ventilated and fed and could not affect their environment at all,<br />

which made it impossible to them to establish a link between their behaviour and<br />

the reward from outside. It was not possible for these rats to learn through operant<br />

conditioning. Birbaumer hypothesized that when CLIS patients can not affect<br />

their environment for extended periods <strong>of</strong> time, the lack <strong>of</strong> reward from the environment<br />

might cause extinction <strong>of</strong> the ability to voluntarily produce brain signals. If<br />

this hypothesis is correct, it supports the idea that BCI training should begin before<br />

loss <strong>of</strong> muscle control, or as soon as possible afterwards [38]. It should be noted<br />

that no patients have proceeded with BCI training from the locked-in state into the<br />

complete locked-in state. Maybe a LIS patient who is able to control a BCI is very<br />

capable <strong>of</strong> transferring this ability in the complete locked-in state.<br />

Other ideas have been put forward as to why CLIS patients seem unable to<br />

learn BCI control. Hill and colleagues [38] proposed that it might be very difficult<br />

for a long term paralyzed patient to imagine movements as is required for SMR<br />

self-regulation or that it is impossible to self-regulate SMR, because <strong>of</strong> physiological<br />

changes due to the disease. Also, general cognitive deficits, lack <strong>of</strong> attention,<br />

fatigue, lack <strong>of</strong> motivation or depression have been mentioned as possible factors<br />

that hamper BCI learning [38, 41].<br />

Neumann [42, 43] and Neumann and Kübler [44] describe how attention, motivation<br />

and mood can affect BCI performance <strong>of</strong> ALS patients. Nijboer and colleagues<br />

also found that mood and motivation influences BCI performance in healthy subjects<br />

[45] and in ALS patients (data is being prepared for publication). It is impossible<br />

to ask CLIS patients how motivated they are to train with the BCI, but data from<br />

communicating ALS patients show that ALS patients are generally more motivated<br />

to perform well with BCI than healthy study participants. Personal experience with<br />

ALS patients and healthy subjects is compatible with this conclusion. ALS patients<br />

who participated in our studies during the past few years were highly intrinsically<br />

motivated, because they see the BCI as a final option when communication with the<br />

muscles is no longer possible. ALS patients were also very willing to participate in<br />

our studies, despite extensive training demands, discomfort <strong>of</strong> putting the electrode

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!