05.01.2013 Views

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

142 <strong>Perceptual</strong> <strong>Coherence</strong><br />

frequency <strong>and</strong> spatial orientation or by frequency <strong>and</strong> time. For both there<br />

are trade-offs between the frequency resolution <strong>and</strong> the time or orientation<br />

resolution.<br />

Experimental Physiological <strong>and</strong> <strong>Perceptual</strong> Outcomes<br />

Using Natural Stimuli<br />

Even though the environment may be made up of many processes that can<br />

be modeled in terms of 1/f c power laws <strong>and</strong> models of cortical cells display<br />

a sparse firing distribution, that does not mean that (1) the neural cells actually<br />

are organized to yield sparse coding, <strong>and</strong> (2) people are sensitive to<br />

the self-similarity of the processes. Moreover, although it may be true that<br />

people can distinguish between auditory <strong>and</strong> visual fractal representations<br />

based on different values of the frequency exponent, that still does not necessarily<br />

imply that people perceive the fractal structure itself.<br />

Experimental Physiological Outcomes<br />

Vinje <strong>and</strong> Gallant (2000, 2002) created a sequence of visual images that simulated<br />

what a monkey would see if it scanned a static natural scene <strong>and</strong><br />

recorded from neurons located in area V1 of two awake macaque monkeys.<br />

Vinje <strong>and</strong> Gallant were particularly interested in how the firing of a classical<br />

receptive area is influenced by stimulation of the surrounding area (the nonclassical<br />

receptive field described in chapter 2). The theoretical <strong>and</strong> computation<br />

approaches described above hypothesize that area V1 uses a sparse code<br />

to efficiently represent natural scenes (remember, natural scenes have a great<br />

deal of built-in redundancy, so that sparse codes would be effective). Vinje<br />

<strong>and</strong> Gallant hypothesized that stimulation of the nonclassical response field<br />

increased the degree of sparseness. The results demonstrated that indeed a<br />

sparse code best represented the firing patterns found in the roughly 60 neurons.<br />

Moreover, as the nonclassical receptive field increased in size, the<br />

sparseness of the firing increased due to the nonlinear receptive field interactions<br />

(compare figure 3.15C to figure 3.15B). Individual neurons became<br />

more selective in responding to complex stimuli, so that the kurtosis of the<br />

firing distribution increased. Stimulation of the nonclassical receptive fields<br />

reduces the response to noise more than it reduces the firing to stimulus properties,<br />

<strong>and</strong> that increases the efficiency of the coding. Vinje <strong>and</strong> Gallant found<br />

that the degree of sparseness was the same for sequences of black-<strong>and</strong>-white<br />

gratings <strong>and</strong> natural movies, suggesting that it is the correlated energy at different<br />

orientations in both types of images that created the sparse coding.<br />

Perez-Orive et al. (2002) presented a particularly compelling example of<br />

sparse coding in the olfactory system of locusts. Roughly 90,000 receptor<br />

neurons converge on about 1,000 excitatory projection neurons in the ol-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!