05.01.2013 Views

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The Perception of Quality: Visual Color 323<br />

test <strong>and</strong> surround will also remain the same. Thus, the constancy of relative<br />

ratios should lead to color constancy.<br />

Kraft <strong>and</strong> Brainard (1999) equalized the local surround by using different<br />

combinations of illumination <strong>and</strong> surface reflectance. The logic of the<br />

experiments is that if we illuminate a gray paper with two different illuminants<br />

but somehow balance the excitation due to illumination × reflectance,<br />

observers will not be able to achieve color constancy because they cannot<br />

untangle the illumination from the surface reflectance. In the first combination,<br />

the background wall was gray cardboard, <strong>and</strong> the illumination in the<br />

entire chamber was a neutral light producing a uniform reflectance spectrum<br />

(figure 7.8, local surround A). In the second combination, the background<br />

wall was blue <strong>and</strong> the illumination in the entire chamber was<br />

orange-red, which also produced a neutral uniform reflectance spectrum<br />

from the back wall (local surround B). Thus, the reflection from the back<br />

wall surrounding the test patch was identical in the two conditions, although<br />

the light reflected from the test patch <strong>and</strong> all of the other surfaces in<br />

the chamber differed. (Note that the difference between the local surround<br />

condition <strong>and</strong> the minimizing control condition described in the above<br />

paragraph is the inclusion of the colored surfaces <strong>and</strong> objects in the local<br />

surround condition. In both conditions, the local surround is matched.) If<br />

the local surround is the only cue for constancy, then equalizing the local<br />

surround should eliminate any constancy. But if the local surround is but<br />

one of several cues <strong>and</strong> if observers can use the more orange appearance of<br />

other objects in the chamber to infer the illuminant, then constancy should<br />

simply decrease. In fact, constancy did decrease to roughly 0.50, although<br />

this is still relatively good.<br />

The second part of the experiment investigated the role of the global surround.<br />

The notion parallels that for the local surround, but here the three<br />

cone responses to the test patch are compared to the three cone responses<br />

averaged across the entire scene. On this basis, Kraft <strong>and</strong> Brainard (1999)<br />

compared neutral illumination of the chamber (spatial mean A) to pale red<br />

illumination that equated the average cone responses across the entire<br />

scene (spatial mean B). In this condition, if subjects are judging the test<br />

patch in terms of the entire scene, then constancy should be zero, but if they<br />

are making use of other cues, then constancy should simply decrease. The<br />

constancy did decrease to 0.40, demonstrating that a difference in the<br />

global surround is not the sole mechanism for constancy.<br />

The third part of the experiment investigated the role of absorption from<br />

the most intense region of the scene, the white areas described previously.<br />

In one condition, the yellow frame was illuminated by a neutral light<br />

(maximum flux A), while in the second condition a magenta frame was<br />

illuminated by a yellow light (maximum flux B). For both conditions, the<br />

background was the identical dark gray cardboard, <strong>and</strong> all the other surfaces

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!