05.01.2013 Views

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Auditory <strong>and</strong> Visual Segmentation 409<br />

These timings are consistent with the temporal integration window described<br />

above.<br />

Recanzone (2003) has further demonstrated the auditory capture of temporal<br />

patterns. In the baseline conditions, a st<strong>and</strong>ard series of four lights or<br />

four tones was presented at the rate of four elements per second. Then a<br />

comparison series was presented at rates between 3.5 <strong>and</strong> 4.5 elements per<br />

second, <strong>and</strong> the participants indicated whether the rate of the second sequence<br />

was faster or slower than the st<strong>and</strong>ard. Participants could judge differences<br />

in auditory rate far better than differences in visual rate (i.e., the<br />

difference threshold for auditory rate was smaller).<br />

The interesting conditions involved the simultaneous presentation of the<br />

auditory <strong>and</strong> visual sequences. In the st<strong>and</strong>ard sequences, the auditory <strong>and</strong><br />

visual stimuli were always presented synchronously at four elements per<br />

second. In the comparison sequences, the auditory <strong>and</strong> visual stimuli were<br />

presented at different rates (see figure 9.16). Here, the participants were<br />

told to ignore either the auditory or visual sequence <strong>and</strong> make their judgments<br />

of rate only on the basis of the other. On those trials in which listeners<br />

were told to attend to the auditory sequence <strong>and</strong> ignore the visual<br />

sequence, listeners were able to ignore the visual sequence <strong>and</strong> based their<br />

judgments solely on the rate of the auditory sequence. In contrast, on those<br />

trials in which listeners were told to attend to the visual sequence <strong>and</strong> ignore<br />

the auditory sequence, they were unable to do so. The rate of the supposedly<br />

ignored auditory sequence determined their judgments of visual<br />

rate. In other words, the timing of the visual lights was perceived to be<br />

equal to that of the supposedly ignored auditory stimuli. The arrows in figure<br />

9.16 depict these two outcomes. There were, however, rate limits; if the<br />

auditory stimuli were presented at twice the rate of the visual stimuli (8/s<br />

versus 4/s) there was no visual capture: Observers were able to accurately<br />

judge the visual rate.<br />

These results confirm previous work demonstrating auditory “driving”<br />

of the perceived visual flashing rate. For example, Shipley (1964) required<br />

participants to adjust the rate of the auditory stimulus so that it appeared<br />

just different than the rate of the visual stimulus. If the rate of the flashing<br />

light was set at 10 Hz, the mismatch was not detected until the auditory rate<br />

decreased to 7 Hz or increased to 22 Hz. Based on these results <strong>and</strong> those<br />

of Recanzone (2003) described above, it appears that the auditory stimulus<br />

can drive the visual rate from one-half to two times the actual flashing rate<br />

of the visual input.<br />

All of these experiments used nonmeaningful stimuli, so that it is<br />

impossible to determine whether the participants perceived the tones<br />

<strong>and</strong> lights as coming from a single object or not, or whether the strength<br />

of the illusion depended on the compellingness of the connection between<br />

the auditory <strong>and</strong> visual inputs. My guess is that auditory driving occurs

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!