05.01.2013 Views

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

experience. Erickson (2003) found that when untrained listeners <strong>and</strong> choral<br />

directors judged the similarity between sung vowels of classical singers,<br />

the first two dimensions were identical: pitch <strong>and</strong> frequency of the spectral<br />

centroid. The choral directors did, however, also use the vibrato frequency<br />

to judge similarity.<br />

I would argue that the lack of effect due to experience is due to the task<br />

itself. Regardless of experience, all subjects were judging the difference in<br />

terms of acoustic differences <strong>and</strong> not in terms of the source or musical implications.<br />

Only two sounds were presented at one time, <strong>and</strong> the pairs were<br />

presented in a r<strong>and</strong>om order. Listeners were instructed to judge the difference<br />

between the sounds <strong>and</strong> not commit the stimulus error of judging the<br />

difference between the sources. This outcome can be contrasted to the typical<br />

results comparing untrained <strong>and</strong> trained listeners in tasks involving musical<br />

tonality. In such tasks, untrained listeners make their judgments in<br />

terms of the difference in pitch, while trained listeners make their judgments<br />

in terms of the musical intervals. Thus, untrained listeners are treating<br />

pitch <strong>and</strong> timbre differences acoustically, while explicit training allows<br />

trained listeners to treat intervals as entities (Krumhansl, 2000).<br />

The effect of musical training on identification <strong>and</strong> discrimination tasks<br />

has also been small (e.g., Brown et al., 2001). Erickson, Perry, <strong>and</strong> H<strong>and</strong>el<br />

(2001) found that experienced singers were only slightly more accurate in<br />

detecting which of three vowels sung at different pitches was produced by<br />

the “oddball” singer. Moreover, this difference disappeared when the range<br />

in pitches exceeded one octave. In similar fashion, Lutfi, Oh, Storm, <strong>and</strong><br />

Alex<strong>and</strong>er (2005) found no differences among practiced listeners, musicians,<br />

<strong>and</strong> nonmusicians in distinguishing between actual <strong>and</strong> synthesized<br />

impact sounds.<br />

Timbre as a Source Variable<br />

The Perception of Quality: Auditory Timbre 361<br />

We would expect that there would be a close coupling between the physical<br />

properties of objects <strong>and</strong> the acoustical properties of the resulting sounds<br />

<strong>and</strong> that that coupling should enable the listener to identify the source. This<br />

emphasis on identifying the source of a sound may result in different ways<br />

of listening than when a subject is listening for the quality or the meaning<br />

of the sound. Of course, the source itself can have inherent meaning: something<br />

to run toward or run away from, for example.<br />

Moreover, we would expect the context, the prior probabilities of different<br />

sounds, higher-level cognitive factors such as memory <strong>and</strong> attention,<br />

<strong>and</strong> individual differences among the listeners to be important factors in<br />

determining the ability to identify the source. Although there seem to be<br />

consistent acoustic properties that correlate with similarity judgments, this

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!