05.01.2013 Views

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

408 <strong>Perceptual</strong> <strong>Coherence</strong><br />

then any pair of inputs will generate different output excitations, depending<br />

on the amount of temporal overlap of the pair. 5<br />

Why then do we perceive simultaneity at most distances? One possibility<br />

is that there is a long temporal integration window. Based on single-cell<br />

recordings, if the auditory <strong>and</strong> visual neural signals occur within 100 ms of<br />

each other, that input will be sufficient to fire neural cells that integrate firings<br />

in different modalities (Meredith, Clemo, & Stein, 1987). This implies<br />

that the source of the auditory <strong>and</strong> visual stimulus energy could be +/−<br />

30 m or more from each other, based on the overlap of activation patterns<br />

(Meredith et al., 1987). Sugita <strong>and</strong> Suzuki (2003) have suggested an alternative<br />

to a wide integration duration, namely that the integration region<br />

shifts as a function of the perceived distance of the source. In their task, observers<br />

judged whether an LED light source positioned from 1 to 50 m in<br />

front of them was presented before or after a burst of white noise presented<br />

by headphones. They found that when the LEDs were 1 m away, the noise<br />

needed to be delayed by 5 ms to be perceived as synchronous, but if the<br />

LEDs were 40 m away, the noise had to be delayed by 106 ms to be perceived<br />

as synchronous. The participants thus expected the sound to occur<br />

later relative to the light as the distance increased. There are huge but stable<br />

individual differences in this sort of task. With 17 participants, the range of<br />

synchrony judgments was 170 ms (Stone et al., 2001).<br />

Perceived Rhythmic Synchrony (Temporal<br />

Ventriloquism)<br />

In the research described below, the auditory input is a series of discrete<br />

short tones while the visual input may be a continuous light or a series of<br />

discrete short light flashes. Here, the onset of a visual target is perceived to<br />

occur at the onset of a sound even if the timings are quite different. This illusionary<br />

percept has been termed temporal ventriloquism to create a parallel<br />

to the classical term spatial ventriloquism, in which a sound is perceived<br />

to come from the spatial location of the visual object. 6<br />

One type of temporal ventriloquism occurs when the amplitude modulation<br />

of a tone induces an illusionary modulation of a light. For example, if a<br />

single visual flash is presented simultaneously with two or more short auditory<br />

beeps, the light flash appears to oscillate on <strong>and</strong> off two times (Shams,<br />

Kamitani, & Shimojo, 2000). The oscillation occurs even if the beep onset<br />

is delayed by 70 ms, but disappears if the onset delay is 100 ms or more.<br />

5. This is yet another example of a population code, analogous to those for orientation or<br />

auditory frequency. The perceptual information is found in the distribution of the responses,<br />

not the magnitude of any single output.<br />

6. For temporal ventriloquism, the observer’s task is to judge the occurrence or rhythm of<br />

an event. For spatial ventriloquism, the observer’s task is to judge the location of an event.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!