05.01.2013 Views

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Transformation of Sensory Information Into <strong>Perceptual</strong> Information 71<br />

modulation that characterizes speech, animal communication, <strong>and</strong> music.<br />

Neurons in the cochlear nucleus can amplify the depth of the amplitude<br />

modulation over a wide range of sound levels. Moreover, the effect of<br />

background noise is small, <strong>and</strong> there are neurons in which the response to<br />

the amplitude modulation actually is enhanced in background noise. The<br />

rate of amplitude modulation is coded by the synchronous responses (i.e.,<br />

the phase-locked responses) to the modulation.<br />

The inferior colliculus integrates almost all the ascending acoustic information<br />

<strong>and</strong> determines the form in which information is conveyed to the<br />

auditory cortex. The receptive fields of many neurons match those from<br />

lower nuclei in the auditory pathways <strong>and</strong> also match the results from psychophysical<br />

experiments using the same paradigms. The frequency b<strong>and</strong>widths<br />

are similar <strong>and</strong> the receptive fields are relatively constant across<br />

changes in intensity.<br />

This differs from the receptive fields found at the auditory brainstem.<br />

There, the b<strong>and</strong>width greatly increases at higher intensities. This suggests<br />

that the ability to resolve frequencies should decrease at higher intensities<br />

because the b<strong>and</strong>widths of the tuning curves measure the ability to resolve<br />

differences between frequencies, but that is not the case. We can measure<br />

the behavioral ability to resolve <strong>and</strong> integrate frequencies by determining<br />

the frequency range of a noise that masks the detection of a tone at the<br />

middle frequency of the noise. This range has been termed the critical b<strong>and</strong>.<br />

Noise energy outside this frequency range does not affect the detectability<br />

of the tone. This experimental procedure yields b<strong>and</strong>widths that are roughly<br />

one third of an octave wide <strong>and</strong> are basically constant across a wide range of<br />

intensities. Frequencies that fall within one critical b<strong>and</strong> are not resolved<br />

(into distinguishable frequencies) <strong>and</strong> are summed. It is critically important<br />

to underst<strong>and</strong> that there are many critical b<strong>and</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> there is a great deal of<br />

Figure 2.24. Panel (A) is a schematic drawing of place × timing models. The<br />

cochlea performs an initial frequency analysis <strong>and</strong> then the outputs at the different<br />

frequencies are analyzed separately, the place component. The timings between the<br />

spikes of each such output are autocorrelated (equivalent to measuring the frequency<br />

distribution of the intervals), <strong>and</strong> the excitation is assumed to be proportional<br />

to the size of the autocorrelation at each delay interval. The summary<br />

autocorrelation histogram is calculated by adding the autocorrelations across receptors,<br />

as illustrated at the right. Figure courtesy of Dr. Peter Cariani. Panel (B) is an<br />

example of a summary correlogram based on the first ten harmonics of a 100 Hz<br />

sound. The evenly spaced peaks at each frequency are due to the autocorrelations<br />

at multiples of the period. The summary correlogram isolates the 10 ms period of<br />

the 100 Hz fundamental. From “Virtual Pitch <strong>and</strong> Phase Sensitivity of a Computer<br />

Model of the Auditory Periphery. I. Pitch Identification,” by R. Meddis <strong>and</strong><br />

M. J. Hewitt, 1991, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89, 2866-2882.<br />

Copyright 1991 by the American Institute of Physics. Reprinted with permission.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!