05.01.2013 Views

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

372 <strong>Perceptual</strong> <strong>Coherence</strong><br />

experiment, often are not clearly connected to the sound production process,<br />

<strong>and</strong> much of the time do not seem intuitive to me. Maybe that should<br />

be expected because of the complexity of the stimuli due to the large number<br />

of vibration modes of the source <strong>and</strong> filter <strong>and</strong> the number of unique receptor<br />

cells, <strong>and</strong> because listeners will make use of any acoustic property<br />

that works in a context.<br />

A recent approach to source identification, termed physically inspired<br />

modeling, may provide a systematic way to investigate the perception of<br />

simple rigid objects. Physically inspired modeling makes use of the vibration<br />

wave equations for the motions of simple bars to synthesize the sounds<br />

of those objects. The advantage of this approach is that it allows the experimenter<br />

to vary the parameters of the vibration modes (e.g., the decay rates)<br />

<strong>and</strong> thereby isolate the properties that listeners actually use to identify the<br />

objects. For example, Lutfi (2001) synthesized the sound of freely vibrating<br />

hollow <strong>and</strong> solid bars <strong>and</strong> required listeners to distinguish between the two<br />

types. The first vibration mode of hollow bars decays more quickly, is<br />

louder, <strong>and</strong> has a lower frequency than the first mode of solid bars. Some<br />

listeners used decay <strong>and</strong> frequency while others used only frequency to<br />

make their judgments. By comparing the wave equations to discrimination,<br />

Lutfi was able to show that the limits of human sensitivity (i.e., internal<br />

noise) can account for the errors made as the intrinsic acoustic relationships<br />

are perturbed.<br />

Moreover, I have equated color constancy with the ability to predict<br />

sound quality at one pitch <strong>and</strong> loudness from the sound quality of the same<br />

object at a different pitch <strong>and</strong> loudness. At least at a superficial level, that<br />

seems to be a correct analogy. To achieve color constancy, the observer<br />

must remove the effects of the change in illumination to capture the unchanging<br />

surface reflectance. To achieve sound source constancy, the listener<br />

must remove the effects of changes in pitch <strong>and</strong> loudness to capture<br />

the source resonances. But the source filter resonances change at different<br />

excitation frequencies so that the listener can be, in effect, faced with a<br />

different sound object when the excitation frequencies are widely different.<br />

Without sounds at intermediate frequencies to characterize the connecting<br />

transformation, it would be impossible to judge whether two sounds<br />

come from the same object. Perhaps the analogy holds within overlapping<br />

frequency ranges of about one octave where the source filters are fairly<br />

constant.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!