05.01.2013 Views

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Transformation of Sensory Information Into <strong>Perceptual</strong> Information 85<br />

fields. The authors pointed out that for natural stimuli such as the zebra<br />

finch songs used here, there are strong spectral <strong>and</strong> temporal correlations.<br />

For example, the overall amplitude envelopes of a set of frequency b<strong>and</strong>s<br />

(neglecting the fine variations within the envelope) are correlated over time<br />

so that the onsets <strong>and</strong> offsets of one b<strong>and</strong> can predict the corresponding<br />

timing of another b<strong>and</strong>. This suggests that the optimal stimulus representation<br />

should remove such correlations in order to reduce the redundancies.<br />

However, Theunissen et al. (2000) found that the correlations between<br />

frequency b<strong>and</strong>s did affect the spectral-temporal receptive fields because<br />

simulated versions of songs that balanced the average frequency distribution<br />

<strong>and</strong> rhythmic distribution did not yield firing patterns identical to those<br />

of the actual sounds that contained the correlations. Two typical neurons<br />

of this sort are shown in figure 2.32. Neuron (A) appears to be tuned to a<br />

downward-moving frequency edge, while neuron (B) appears to be tuned<br />

to a nonoverlapping combination of frequencies between 1500 <strong>and</strong> 2500 Hz,<br />

followed by frequencies between 4000 <strong>and</strong> 6500 Hz.<br />

Figure 2.32. The response of two auditory neurons to real conspecific songs<br />

shows a distinct receptive field structure (C <strong>and</strong> D). The response, however, is very<br />

weak to a sequence of tones that has the same frequency distribution as the real<br />

conspecific songs, but a different sequence <strong>and</strong> rhythm (A <strong>and</strong> B). From “Spectral-<br />

Temporal Receptive Fields of Nonlinear Auditory Neurons Obtained Using Natural<br />

Sounds,” by F. E. Theunissen, K. Sen, <strong>and</strong> A. J. Doupe, 2000, Journal of Neuroscience,<br />

20, 2315–2331.Copyright 2000 by the Society for Neuroscience. Reprinted<br />

with permission.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!