05.01.2013 Views

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The Transition Between Noise <strong>and</strong> Structure 155<br />

K = 1: The first-order statistic is the probability that any dot in the array<br />

will have a certain brightness or luminance. In B. F. J. Green et al. (1957),<br />

there were only two levels of brightness (i.e., white or black) so that<br />

Pr(white) = 1 − Pr(black). Julesz (1962) used two to four levels of brightness<br />

(black, dark gray, light gray, white). Different probability distributions<br />

vary the overall brightness <strong>and</strong> contrast of the array, <strong>and</strong> this variable has<br />

been termed spatial density or tonal quality. Three r<strong>and</strong>om textures based<br />

on different probability distributions of 10 different gray levels are shown<br />

in figure 4.1. Different probability distributions create textures that yield<br />

easy segregation.<br />

K = 2: The second-order statistic is the conditional probability between<br />

pairs of dots. Simply put, given the brightness of one dot, is the probability<br />

of the brightness of a second dot different than its probability of occurrence<br />

according to the K = 1 statistic? In figure 4.2, three examples of K = 2<br />

Figure 4.1. Texture differences due to K = 1 statistics of the reflectance of the<br />

individual squares in each 12 × 12 matrix. The y axis gives the percentages of each<br />

of the 10 different reflectance values. Differences in the K = 1 statistic generate<br />

rapid segmentation between the three matrices arrayed vertically, although the<br />

boundary is soft.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!