05.01.2013 Views

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

Perceptual Coherence : Hearing and Seeing

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

368 <strong>Perceptual</strong> <strong>Coherence</strong><br />

sounds, a third rubbing sounds, <strong>and</strong> so on (as in Gaver, 1993, above). Then<br />

the responses of all the listeners to one sound were put in the appropriate<br />

categories. If all listeners describe the sound as the same type of event,<br />

then all responses will fall in one category, <strong>and</strong> the uncertainty will be zero.<br />

If all of the descriptions are of different types of events <strong>and</strong> occur equally<br />

in all the different categories, the uncertainty will be the maximum value<br />

possible. Simply put, causal uncertainty is an indicant of how many different<br />

events could have produced the sound. The measure of causal uncertainty<br />

was highly correlated to the mean time it took to identify the sound,<br />

<strong>and</strong> several acoustic features (e.g., presence of harmonics) also were correlated<br />

to identification time. Thus, both the causal uncertainty <strong>and</strong> presence<br />

of specific acoustic features influence identification.<br />

Ballas (1993) used several methods to assess the effect of familiarity.<br />

First, based on a written description (not the sound itself ), listeners rated<br />

their familiarity with the object. Second, to measure the a priori probability<br />

that the sound occurred in the natural environment, participants were asked<br />

to report the first sound they heard when a timer r<strong>and</strong>omly activated. On the<br />

whole, there is a weak relationship between ecological frequency (method<br />

2) <strong>and</strong> causal uncertainty. If a sound occurs frequently, we might expect listeners<br />

to have more chances to discover the relevant acoustic cues. One<br />

possible reason for the weak relationship is that most of the sounds that occurred<br />

frequently were background sounds (air in heating ducts) that listeners<br />

normally do not pay attention to.<br />

Ballas (1993) concluded that identification is best conceptualized as<br />

arising from information in different domains, rather than arising from a<br />

single measure of some sort. The maximum prediction of the identification<br />

time included four types of data: (1) temporal acoustic properties (similar<br />

spectral bursts in noncontinuous sounds); (2) spectral acoustic properties<br />

(average frequency of spectrum); (3) amplitude envelope (ratio of burst duration<br />

to total duration); <strong>and</strong> (4) ecological (frequency of occurrence). This<br />

is the same “no smoking gun” conclusion coming from experiments on<br />

color constancy.<br />

Source Constancy: Multiple Sounds From One Source<br />

There is hardly any cross-referencing between the research on timbre as a<br />

sound quality <strong>and</strong> the research on source identification, even though both<br />

kinds of studies arrive at nearly the same temporal <strong>and</strong> spectral properties.<br />

The research on timbre quality has emphasized human sounds (e.g., musical<br />

instruments, speaking <strong>and</strong> singing voices) that can be produced at<br />

widely different fundamental frequencies <strong>and</strong> amplitudes. The research on<br />

source identification has concentrated on the identification of a single<br />

sound (even if the sound was a set of discrete impacts, e.g., walking) that is

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!