25.12.2013 Views

0 INTRODUCTION

0 INTRODUCTION

0 INTRODUCTION

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

6. FINDINGS<br />

Figure 6. Frequency of Romance nominalizations according to syntactic function<br />

A possible reason for the lower use of –ing nominalizations functioning as subjects,<br />

objects and predicatives if compared to Romance nominalizations is the fact that most –ing<br />

nominalizations found in my data have only post-head dependents. As Fanego (2006,<br />

2007) has pointed out, verbal –ing nominalizations having only post-head dependents<br />

became available as subjects, objects and predicatives only in the seventeenth and<br />

eighteenth centuries, except for a few scattered earlier instances. In other words, verbal<br />

–ing formations are rarely used in those positions until the middle of the period under<br />

analysis here, and thus are rarely recorded in my data.<br />

However, the most powerful reason for the lower frequency of nominalizations in the<br />

syntactic positions mentioned above is that both –ing formations and Romance<br />

nominalizations appear overwhelmingly as complements of a preposition (997 of all –ing<br />

nominalizations [72% of the total]; and 814 of all Romance nominalizations [56% of the<br />

total]) (see Figures 5 and 6 above). If verbal –ing formations are considered in isolation,<br />

their preference for being used as obliques is much stronger (363 verbal formations [98.6%<br />

of the total]). Ultimately, as Houston (1989: 176) and Fanego (1996) note, the frequency<br />

184

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!