04.06.2014 Views

Volu m e II - Purdue University Calumet

Volu m e II - Purdue University Calumet

Volu m e II - Purdue University Calumet

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

She [the child in the story] should) scream. I wouldn’t want my uncle to put his hand on<br />

my bottom. I’d be uncomfortable. I wouldn’t kick him, but, then, my uncle wouldn’t do that, I<br />

don’t think. If he’d come over again, someone should just tell<br />

him Mary is not home, that she’s<br />

at a friend’s house. If I was home, I’d lock my<br />

door. She probably felt embarrassed. She could<br />

hop out of bed and run. She shouldn’t listen to her uncle when he does something wrong. See, grownups<br />

can tell you what to do if you do the wrong thing, but you tell yourself (what to do) if<br />

the grown-ups do the wrong thing. She could slap him, bite him, and tell her parents. It just<br />

doesn’t seem right for an uncle to do something like that… (Burkhardt, 1991).<br />

While the response of the nine-year-old child is considerably more developed and sensible than that<br />

of the six-year-old, in most actual situations of childhood sexual abuse, children do not respond and report,<br />

and sometimes fail to even recognize, the abuse the way their responses may suggest (Child 2011). This<br />

effect is due in large part to their cognitive vulnerability:<br />

Children are not only physically unsuited to resist adult advances, but their reasoning<br />

capabilities may be inadequate to meet the social demand of responding to an adult’s abusive use of<br />

authority. Also, it is possible that even when a child<br />

might know that a response is needed, he<br />

or she may be unable to determine or execute an effective response. Research in cognitive development,<br />

particularly in the realm of social cognition, has made strides in identifying the qualities and<br />

quantitative differences between childhood reasoning and adult logic (Burkhardt 1991).<br />

Additionally, there is a question regarding what children are instructed to do in response to an<br />

inappropriate directive by an adult. For example, “Say No and Go” was popular in the 1980s. Parents would<br />

instruct their children that, in a “stranger danger” situation, the child should say “no,” and “go,” or flee the<br />

situation as fast as possible (Burkhardt 1986). Similarly, it is difficult to determine how effective a<br />

resistance and reporting strategy might actually be. One element of children’s compliance with perpetrators<br />

and their failure to resist and report sexual abuse may have to do with the degree of authority attributed to<br />

the perpetrator.<br />

Such differences account for the gap between what a child knows and what s/he understands,<br />

which was the focus of Burkhardt’s study. A child may know it’s wrong to be touched inappropriately, but<br />

does not necessarily know why, except for the fact that a parent instructed the child to believe this, or that<br />

such touching may feel uncomfortable. But, if the child does know what to do, why is the rate of childhood<br />

sexual abuse so high? The child may theoretically know what should be done, but may not be able to<br />

206

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!