04.06.2014 Views

Volu m e II - Purdue University Calumet

Volu m e II - Purdue University Calumet

Volu m e II - Purdue University Calumet

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

“Do kids have the right to disobey sometimes? Like when?”: this question seemed to be the breaking point<br />

for the seven-year-old participant. Up until this point, all of her answers implied that she was wellrehearsed,<br />

aware, and insightful of the topic—possibly intrinsically so. Up until this point, it seemed as<br />

though the responses of the seven-year-old had completely proven the researcher‟s hypothesis wrong, and a<br />

seven-year-old child is indeed able to understand the sexual nature of the vignette and to understand when<br />

an adult has proper authority and when s/he does not. The seven-year-old child had validated all of her<br />

“why‟s” with logical, seemingly insightful responses. Yet, for the final question: “Do kids have the right to<br />

disobey sometimes? Like when?” the child gives a simple answer: no. Unfortunately, when prompted to<br />

generalize her overall experience of the vignette—the exact resistance strategy which carries hope of<br />

prevention of sexual abuse for younger children—the child fails to do so. It seems that without a leading or<br />

suggestive question, the child is still limited in her cognitive ability to make an assessment of a complex<br />

situation. The mark of being able to make logic of and properly assess something is evaluation: in this case,<br />

being able to generalize conclusions derived from assessment. The child fails to do so.<br />

The researcher‟s hypothesis predicted that the older children would have significantly more<br />

discerning responses—reflecting a deeper understanding and a higher cognitive ability.<br />

Two older children were interviewed. The first participant, a male of age ten, assumed without hesitation<br />

that the problem in the story was that Tommy had lost his puppy. He believed that Tommy should “put up<br />

signs saying „lost dog,‟ so people can find his dog.” The participant believed that Tommy will “go try to find<br />

[the man‟s] dog, because he loves his dog.” Likewise, the child also believed that Tommy should look at the<br />

picture of the puppy, as the man had instructed him, “because it‟s a lost puppy”—and therefore it was<br />

“okay” for the man to tell Tommy to look at the picture. However, the ten-year-old boy did not believe<br />

that a man walking down the street has the right to tell children what to do because “it‟s not right. That‟s<br />

just being mean.”<br />

219

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!