02.12.2012 Views

Relativism and Universalism in Linguistics - Fachbereich 10 ...

Relativism and Universalism in Linguistics - Fachbereich 10 ...

Relativism and Universalism in Linguistics - Fachbereich 10 ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Workshop 7 – Ron 195<br />

Prom<strong>in</strong>ence <strong>and</strong> transparency <strong>in</strong> prosodic morphology<br />

Lappe, Sab<strong>in</strong>e<br />

Siegen<br />

lappe@anglistik.uni-siegen.de<br />

Truncatory processes are traditionally held to lack transparency (both semantic <strong>and</strong><br />

morphological). The chief reason for the lack of semantic transparency is that the base form is<br />

taken to be not easily or unambiguously recoverable from the truncated form. Thus, for<br />

example, the English truncated personal name Will may be derived from Wilbert or William;<br />

similarly, English mag may be derived either from magaz<strong>in</strong>e or from magnet. This lack of<br />

recoverability has provided one of the major arguments to dist<strong>in</strong>guish between processes like<br />

truncation as <strong>in</strong>stances of word-creation or extragrammatical morphology on the one h<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

word-formation proper or grammatical word-formation on the other h<strong>and</strong>.<br />

In this paper I will look at recoverability of base words <strong>in</strong> truncatory processes from an<br />

empirical perspective. It will be shown that from this perspective, recoverability - <strong>and</strong>, hence,<br />

transparency - is much less a problem for the <strong>in</strong>terpretation of truncated words than hitherto<br />

assumed. The ma<strong>in</strong> bulk of data will come from English (Lappe 2005), where truncatory<br />

processes - especially personal name truncation - are highly productive. The truncatory<br />

patterns to be considered will be simple name truncation (e.g. Will < William), hypocoristic<br />

formation (e.g. Willy < William), <strong>and</strong> clipp<strong>in</strong>g of other words, both unsuffixed (e.g. mag <<br />

magnet, celeb < celebrity) <strong>and</strong> suffixed (e.g. chrissie < chrysanthemum).<br />

On the basis of a corpus compris<strong>in</strong>g more than 3,000 base-derivative pairs, we will first of all<br />

see that, <strong>in</strong> practice, truncated forms pos<strong>in</strong>g recoverability problems are the exception rather<br />

than the rule. Specifically, the assumption that truncation produces homophonous truncated<br />

forms whose bases are unclear is true for only a very small m<strong>in</strong>ority of cases <strong>in</strong> the corpus.<br />

Secondly, we will see that truncation processes systematically <strong>in</strong>corporate mechanisms that<br />

facilitate recoverability of base forms, <strong>and</strong>, hence, transparency. These mechanisms crucially<br />

<strong>in</strong>volve the phonological prom<strong>in</strong>ence structure of the base form. For example, English<br />

truncated names systematically vary between two options <strong>in</strong> terms of the segmental material<br />

which they preserve from their bases. More than 90% of the pert<strong>in</strong>ent data preserve either the<br />

first or the ma<strong>in</strong>-stressed syllable of the base. Preservation of nonprom<strong>in</strong>ent material, which is<br />

attested for highly frequent names like Elisabeth (truncated form: Beth), is very rare <strong>in</strong> the<br />

corpus. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, the systematic patterns observed <strong>in</strong> truncation mirror what has been<br />

found <strong>in</strong> psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic studies to be important for word recognition. Truncated forms<br />

systematically preserve those parts of their bases that play a key role <strong>in</strong> lexical access.<br />

In the second part of the paper the empirical f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from English will be compared to<br />

f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs that have emerged from studies of truncatory patterns <strong>in</strong> other languages. We will see<br />

that English does not constitute an isolated case, but that, whenever systematic empirical<br />

studies are available, these studies br<strong>in</strong>g out the prom<strong>in</strong>ence structure of base forms as an<br />

important factor that systematically determ<strong>in</strong>es which part of their bases truncated forms will<br />

reta<strong>in</strong>. The data will come from Spanish (PiZeros 1998), French (Ronneberger-Sibold 1992,<br />

Scullen 1997, Nelson 2003), <strong>and</strong> Italian (Alber 2006, to appear).<br />

With respect to the topic of the workshop, the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs to be presented <strong>in</strong> this paper raise<br />

important theoretical issues. These not only concern the relation between transparency <strong>and</strong><br />

formal compositionality; they also crucially concern the question of what should count as<br />

irregularity <strong>in</strong> morphology.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!