06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

90 <strong>Educability</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Group</strong> <strong>Differences</strong><br />

to those who are not bom with it. To teach intelligence might mean<br />

to point out more or less all the conceivable connections, generalizations,<br />

<strong>and</strong> possible transfer of every item of acquired information,<br />

<strong>and</strong> to elicit <strong>and</strong> reinforce the appropriate responses to these<br />

situations. This could involve teaching more than anyone could<br />

ever learn. Probably no one would live long enough ever to acquire<br />

even a mental age of six. The design of a computer that can ‘learn’<br />

<strong>and</strong> ‘think’ both inductively <strong>and</strong> deductively is necessarily very<br />

different from that of the computer which merely records <strong>and</strong> stores<br />

items of information that can later be elicited <strong>by</strong> specific cues in<br />

a pushbutton fashion.<br />

One of the ways in which scholastic achievement tests differ<br />

from intelligence tests is that at any given point in time, the usual<br />

achievement test scores reflect a relatively larger G or gain<br />

component, intended to assess what had been taught in the recent<br />

past in a particular grade in school. Since various subjects of the<br />

curriculum are newly introduced at different grades, the G component<br />

of achievement tests constitutes a larger proportion in<br />

relation to S than is the case for intelligence tests. The G component<br />

is largely a function of environmental influences, interests, motivation,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the like, acting at any given time. Bloom (1964, pp.<br />

113-19) has reviewed convincing evidence that G is more related<br />

to environmental factors, while C is genetically <strong>and</strong> constitutionally<br />

determined. (Professor Bloom, however, may not concur in this<br />

interpretation.) Thus, accelerated achievement gains brought about<br />

<strong>by</strong> an enriched <strong>and</strong> intensified instructional program generally<br />

‘fade out’ in a few months to a year. Without a strong consolidation<br />

factor, accelerated gains are not maintained without constant<br />

rehearsal of the acquired knowledge or skills. Because variance in<br />

achievement test scores reflects a larger gain component at any<br />

given time than do intelligence tests, which are designed to reflect<br />

the consolidation factor, one should expect populations that differ<br />

on the average on intelligence measures to differ significantly less<br />

on achievement measures at any cross-section in time, <strong>and</strong> this<br />

has been found to be the case (Coleman et al., 1966; <strong>Jensen</strong>,<br />

1971a). Consolidated achievement, however, provided it involves<br />

intellectual skills, should show about the same magnitude of<br />

population differences as are shown <strong>by</strong> intelligence tests.<br />

An interesting difference between scholastic achievement scores<br />

<strong>and</strong> intelligence test scores (including vocabulary) is that the latter

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!