06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The Heritability of Scholastic Achievement 105<br />

assessed; <strong>and</strong> the degree of their mastery <strong>by</strong> any individual will<br />

be largely a function of the amount of time he spends in being<br />

taught <strong>and</strong> in practising the skill. Thus children with quite<br />

different IQs can be shaped up to perform more or less<br />

equally in these elemental skills. If Johnny has trouble with<br />

his spelling or arithmetic, his parents may give him extra tutoring<br />

so that he can more nearly approximate the performance of<br />

his brighter brother. This is not so easily accomplished for<br />

more complex processes such as reading comprehension <strong>and</strong><br />

arithmetic applications, which more nearly resemble IQ in degree<br />

of heritability.<br />

Since school subjects increase in complexity with each grade<br />

level, <strong>and</strong> the consolidated portion of achievement correlates more<br />

<strong>and</strong> more with intelligence, we should expect that the heritability<br />

of scholastic achievement should also increase with age. There is<br />

evidence that this is the case. Husen (1963) found a decreasing<br />

environmental component in achievement variance from fourth<br />

to sixth grade. Increasing achievement differences among children<br />

as they advance in age then increasingly reflect their genetic<br />

intelligence differences. By the last year or two in high school, the<br />

heritability of comprehensive measures of scholastic achievement<br />

(rather than narrow tests which assess only the subject matter<br />

taught in the immediately preceding school year) is quite comparable<br />

to that of measures of general intelligence. This is shown in a<br />

study <strong>by</strong> Nichols (1965) who obtained the scores of large samples<br />

of MZ <strong>and</strong> DZ twins who, as high school juniors (eleventh grade),<br />

had taken the National Merit Scholarship Qualifying Test<br />

(NMSQT), which consists of subtests in English, mathematics,<br />

social studies, natural science, <strong>and</strong> word usage. Nichols notes<br />

that the intraclass correlations (on MZ <strong>and</strong> DZ twins) for the<br />

NMSQT composite score are very similar to correlations for<br />

measures of general intelligence obtained in other twin studies.<br />

These findings, at first glance, might seem to be in conflict with<br />

those of Newman et al. (1937), whose twins wrere adults but<br />

showed quite different correlations for IQ than for achievement.<br />

The reason is most likely that all the twins in Nichols’ sample<br />

were very close to the same age <strong>and</strong> had the same number of years<br />

of schooling. The twins of Newman et al. had quite different<br />

amounts of schooling; for example, one twin went only through<br />

the third grade while her separated co-twin was a college graduate.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!