06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

50 <strong>Educability</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Group</strong> <strong>Differences</strong><br />

growing organism, from the moment of conception, ‘interacts’ with<br />

its environment is a mere truism. It says no more than the fact<br />

that there are no organisms that have existed <strong>and</strong> grown without<br />

an environment. The interactionist position is merely tantamount<br />

to stating that the organism exists. But we already take this for<br />

granted, <strong>and</strong> repeating the assertion that the individual is the result<br />

of ‘the complex interaction of genetic <strong>and</strong> environmental factors’ is<br />

simply stating the obvious. What the population geneticist actually<br />

wishes to know is what proportion of the variation in a particular<br />

trait among individuals is attributable to their genetic differences<br />

<strong>and</strong> what proportion is attributable to differences in their environmental<br />

histories. For the answer to this question one must turn to<br />

the methods of quantitative-genetic analysis. The estimation of<br />

heritability is among these methods. If it were true that environmental<br />

factors so ‘interacted’ with the genetic factors, in the sense<br />

intended <strong>by</strong> the ‘interactionists’, as to completely obliterate any<br />

correlational or predictive connection between an individual’s<br />

genotypes <strong>and</strong> phenotypes, the methods of quantitative genetics<br />

are quite capable of revealing this. If our phenotypic characteristics<br />

or measurements gave no clue as to the ‘genetic value’ of individuals,<br />

it would mean that h2 would be zero, <strong>and</strong>, of course, the correlation<br />

between phenotype <strong>and</strong> genotype (rpG = h) would also be zero.<br />

But in fact, for human intelligence in our present society, most<br />

estimates of h2 fall in the range from 0-60 to 0-90, with an average<br />

close to 0*80 (<strong>Jensen</strong>, 1967, 1969a; Jinks & Fulker, 1970). So much,<br />

then, for the abuse of the term interaction.<br />

But now a clear distinction must be made between two theoretically<br />

quite legitimate though quite different meanings of interaction.<br />

The popular confusion of the two among behavioral scientists is<br />

probably most due to a well-known <strong>and</strong> often reprinted article<br />

‘Heredity, environment <strong>and</strong> the question “How” ’ <strong>by</strong> Anastasi<br />

(1958). Anastasi points out that attempts to determine the proportional<br />

contribution of heredity <strong>and</strong> environment to observed<br />

individual differences in given traits (i.e., heritability estimation)<br />

. . . have usually been based upon the implicit assumption that<br />

hereditary <strong>and</strong> environmental factors combine in an additive<br />

fashion. Both geneticists <strong>and</strong> psychologists have repeatedly<br />

demonstrated, however, that a more tenable hypothesis is that<br />

of interaction [4 references]. In other words, the nature <strong>and</strong>

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!