06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

140 <strong>Educability</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Group</strong> <strong>Differences</strong><br />

can be established from family pedigrees <strong>and</strong> concordance rates<br />

for various degrees of kinship. If samples of the two populations<br />

being compared are sufficiently large in relation to the base rate<br />

frequency of the gene (or the alternate allelic forms of the gene)<br />

in the two populations, then the relative frequency of the gene in<br />

each population can be determined. This has been done with<br />

blood groups <strong>and</strong> many other physical characteristics which show<br />

discrete rather than continuous variation. So far as we know, there<br />

are very few genes that are exclusive to any one population; only<br />

a few of the 70-odd identified blood types fall into this category.<br />

As Thoday (1969, p. 4) notes, ‘Populations of a species do not<br />

differ absolutely, but in the relative frequency of different genotypes.’<br />

Continuous Traits<br />

The characteristics we are most interested in, however, are continuous<br />

variables, like height <strong>and</strong> IQ. Their wide variability over<br />

a continuous range of values is said to be polygenic, that is, a result<br />

of the combined effects of many genes, each one independently<br />

either adding, not adding, or subtracting a small increment of<br />

the trait. Each person’s genotype for a particular trait is comprised<br />

of a r<strong>and</strong>om assortment of the parental genes; thus individuals<br />

inherit genes, not genotypes. Parents can only pass on their genes<br />

to their progeny, not their genotypes. Since many different assortments<br />

<strong>and</strong> combinations of the parental genes are possible, we<br />

see considerable variations both among parents <strong>and</strong> their children<br />

<strong>and</strong> among the children of the same parents. For polygenic traits,<br />

like height <strong>and</strong> IQ, how can w7e prove genetic differences between<br />

populations<br />

Coming back to our Watusi <strong>and</strong> Pygmy example, we can perform<br />

what plant geneticists call a ‘transplant’ <strong>and</strong> animal geneticists call<br />

‘cross-fostering’. That is, we rear members of one population in<br />

the habitat of the other <strong>and</strong> vice versa. Shortly after birth, Pygmy<br />

infants would be given to Watusi for rearing <strong>and</strong> Watusi infants<br />

would be given to Pygmies. Will their adult height come closer<br />

to the mean of the population of their origin or of their adoptive<br />

population If the sample sizes are large enough, this method could<br />

establish with considerable accuracy the relative contributions of<br />

‘nature’ <strong>and</strong> ‘nurture’ to the mean height difference between<br />

Pygmies <strong>and</strong> Watusi. The uncontrolled factor in this case, of

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!