06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Intelligence <strong>and</strong> <strong>Educability</strong> 91<br />

go on increasing steadily throughout the summer months while the<br />

children are not in school, while there is an actual loss in achievement<br />

test scores from the beginning to the end of the summer.<br />

Much of the most recently learned material prior to the summer<br />

vacation has not been sufficiently rehearsed to become consolidated.<br />

The loss is greatest for those school subjects that depend<br />

least upon general intelligence (i.e., the consolidation factor) <strong>and</strong><br />

depend most upon sheer learning <strong>and</strong> memory, such as spelling,<br />

punctuation, grammar, <strong>and</strong> mechanical or computational arithmetic<br />

<strong>and</strong> number facts, as contrasted with reading comprehension<br />

<strong>and</strong> arithmetic concepts (Beggs & Hieronymus, 1968).<br />

Gains in achievement (<strong>and</strong> intelligence test raw scores) are<br />

relatively greater early in learning than later, largely because it is<br />

easier to consolidate gains at the ‘simple’ end of the scale than at<br />

the more complex (‘difficult’) end of the scale of intellectual tasks.<br />

When students simultaneously begin a new course of study, the<br />

diligent but intellectually mediocre students can keep up or even<br />

excel for a time near the beginning of the course; but soon it<br />

becomes increasingly difficult to keep ahead as they progress<br />

further into the complexities of the subject matter. For the less<br />

intelligent students consolidation does not keep up with their gains<br />

to the same extent as for the brighter students. The growth of<br />

intelligence is not reflected mainly <strong>by</strong> an increase in the ability for<br />

simple learning through practice, but in the ability to consolidate<br />

<strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> increasingly complex material. As Leona Tyler<br />

(1965, pp. 78-9) has put it: ‘The child with an IQ of 80 is h<strong>and</strong>icapped<br />

all through school not because he is slow or inept at learning<br />

things which are within the capacity of all the children at his age<br />

level, but because he is never ready to grasp new <strong>and</strong> more complex<br />

ideas at the time when they are ordinarily presented to children<br />

of his age.’ Readiness in large part is the ability to consolidate the<br />

knowledge <strong>and</strong> skills gained through daily learning experiences.<br />

According to our model, at any given point in time, a performance<br />

measure of achievement status (S ) usually reflects more of the<br />

consolidated component (C) than of the gains component (G),<br />

<strong>and</strong> this is increasingly true over the course of development. Since<br />

C is largely genetic <strong>and</strong> stable <strong>and</strong> G is largely environmental <strong>and</strong><br />

r<strong>and</strong>om, an inference from the model is that brighter siblings (<strong>and</strong><br />

twins) should show higher correlations for achievement than duller<br />

siblings. (At any cross-section in time the recent [<strong>and</strong> r<strong>and</strong>om]

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!