06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

246 <strong>Educability</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Group</strong> <strong>Differences</strong><br />

twelfth-grade Indians attend schools in which they are in a<br />

minority. Yet on the non-verbal intelligence test, non-metropolitan<br />

North <strong>and</strong> West Negroes score approximately 0-8 SD (equivalent<br />

to 11-12 IQ points) below the national average, while the Indians<br />

score about 0*1 SD (2-3 IQ points) below the national average.<br />

Even on the verbal ability test the largely bilingual Indians exceed<br />

this Negro group <strong>by</strong> 0-4 SD (about 6 IQ points). Thus these data<br />

lend no support to Bodmer’s <strong>and</strong> Cavalli-Sforza’s conjecture. But<br />

they go on to argue that Coleman’s Indian sample may not<br />

adequately represent the 70 to 80 percent of American Indians<br />

who live on reservations. This is mere surmise, but in any case it is<br />

irrelevant to the point being made <strong>by</strong> these data: they are the very<br />

same Indians who were tested in the Coleman study who also<br />

rated much lower than Negroes on all the environmental indices.<br />

Despite this environmental disadvantage, these same Indians<br />

scored higher than Negroes on the ability <strong>and</strong> achievement tests.<br />

But what about motivation, self-esteem, <strong>and</strong> educational aspirations<br />

These factors are commonly mentioned as explanatory<br />

variables in discussions of Negroes’ mental test <strong>and</strong> scholastic<br />

performance. Gordon (1970, p. 254), for example, states: ‘Moreover,<br />

socially disadvantaged children have been determined <strong>by</strong><br />

several investigators to be less highly motivated <strong>and</strong> to have<br />

lower aspiration for academic <strong>and</strong> vocational achievement than do<br />

their middle <strong>and</strong> upper class school peers.’ Further on in the same<br />

passage, Gordon (p. 255) writes: ‘As important as these attitudes<br />

toward school <strong>and</strong> learning may be, it is in the area of attitude<br />

toward self <strong>and</strong> others that the crucial determinants of achievement<br />

<strong>and</strong> upward mobility may lie. . . .’ Coleman et al. attempted to<br />

take account of these motivational <strong>and</strong> attitudinal factors.<br />

If poor environmental conditions, discrimination, <strong>and</strong> minority<br />

status depress academic motivation, aspiration, <strong>and</strong> self-esteem<br />

as a student, we should expect the Indian students to show lower<br />

ratings on these variables than Negroes. If it is argued that Negroes<br />

suffer greater prejudice, discrimination, <strong>and</strong> the psychological<br />

h<strong>and</strong>icaps they may engender, than do Indians, we should expect<br />

this to show up in Coleman’s motivational <strong>and</strong> attitudinal assessments.<br />

But on a questionnaire of 16 items intended to assess<br />

school-related attitudes, motivation, self-concept, <strong>and</strong> educational<br />

aspirations, Negroes showed higher (more favorable) scores than<br />

Indians; in the twelfth grade, Negroes were higher on 14 of the 16

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!