06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Language Deprivation 289<br />

the presentation of data <strong>and</strong> the statistical analyses thereof make it<br />

probably the best single study available to date. Summarizing the<br />

results on the Developmental Motor Quotient (DMQ), derived from<br />

her infant tests, Bayley (1965, p. 405) reports: ‘The means for the<br />

Negroes are higher at every age (from 1 to 15 months) except 15<br />

months. The difference reaches significance at the 0-01 level of<br />

confidence at months 3, 4, 5, <strong>and</strong> 9 <strong>and</strong> at the 0-05 level at months 7<br />

<strong>and</strong> 12. After 12 months this difference disappears.’ Bayley concludes:<br />

‘Although there is considerable overlap of scores among whites <strong>and</strong><br />

Negroes of the same age, a genetic factor may be operating. That is,<br />

Negroes may be inherently more precocious than whites in their<br />

motor coordinations’ (pp. 408-9). Cravioto (1966, p. 78) has noted<br />

similar results in Indian infants of Guatemala <strong>and</strong> Mexico, commenting<br />

that on the Gesell tests of infant behavior ‘their development<br />

at two or three weeks is similar to that of Western European infants<br />

two or three times as old’. It is also interesting that Orientals<br />

(Chinese-Americans) who, as school age children, equal or exceed<br />

the white population in the most heavily g loaded intelligence tests<br />

<strong>and</strong> in the most abstract scholastic subjects, as infants are significantly<br />

less motorically reactive than white infants, though they show<br />

no significant difference in neuromuscular maturity per se (Freedman<br />

& Freedman, 1969). Chinese <strong>and</strong> Caucasian neonates in the nursery<br />

of the same hospital were ‘tested’ shortly after birth. Marked<br />

differences in reactivity showed up, for example, when a loosely<br />

woven cloth was placed on the face of the supine ba<strong>by</strong>. The typical<br />

Caucasian neonate ‘immmediately struggled to remove the cloth <strong>by</strong><br />

swiping his h<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> turning his face’; the typical Chinese-American<br />

neonate ‘lay impassively, exhibiting few overt motor responses’. This<br />

behavioral difference was significant beyond the 0-0001 level.<br />

‘Similarly, when placed in a prone position, the Chinese infants<br />

frequently lay as placed, with face flat against the bedding, whereas<br />

the Caucasian infants either turned the face to one side or lifted the<br />

head.’ Similar studies of Negro neonates suggest that the three racial<br />

groups lie on a developmental continuum on which the Caucasian<br />

group is more or less intermediate. A related fact is that there is an<br />

inverse relationship throughout the phylogenic hierarchy between<br />

the tendency for multiple births <strong>and</strong> the prolongation of immaturity.<br />

In the course of evolution there has been genetic suppression of<br />

multiple births in all hominids, including man, through natural<br />

selection, although twins <strong>and</strong> other multiple births still occur with<br />

low frequencies. As Harrison, Weiner et al. (1964, p. 91) have noted,<br />

‘Single young is a pre-adaptation for progressively increased<br />

maturation time, <strong>and</strong> in this respect man shows a clear continuity<br />

with the pongids’ (the phylogenically closest group having the most

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!