06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Race <strong>Differences</strong> in Intelligence 183<br />

had to estimate the proportions of MZ <strong>and</strong> DZ twins in her white<br />

<strong>and</strong> Negro samples from the percentages of like-sex <strong>and</strong> unlike-sex<br />

twin pairs. The MZ <strong>and</strong> DZ twin correlations, in turn, are based<br />

on these estimates (along with the correlations for like <strong>and</strong> unlike<br />

sex pairs) <strong>and</strong> are not the directly obtained correlations between<br />

test scores for MZ <strong>and</strong> DZ twins. This estimation procedure used<br />

<strong>by</strong> Scarr-Salapatek greatly enlarges the st<strong>and</strong>ard errors of the<br />

estimated twin correlations <strong>and</strong>, of course, of the heritability<br />

estimates derived therefrom. This serious statistical inadequacy of<br />

the study is not emphasized in Scarr-Salapatek’s report, which<br />

contains no statistical tests of significance of any of her h2 values,<br />

few, if any, of which would have proven sufficiently reliable<br />

statistically to permit rejection of the null hypothesis or of one or<br />

another of the alternative hypotheses under consideration. For<br />

this reason, the present account of Scarr-Salapatek’s findings refers<br />

only to her particular sample <strong>and</strong> procedure. The study permits<br />

no strong inferences concerning the heritability of IQ in white<br />

<strong>and</strong> Negro populations. Unless such studies in the future are done<br />

with sufficiently fastidious methodology <strong>and</strong> large enough samples<br />

to permit strong statistical inference with respect to clearly<br />

formulated models, the mounting data will serve merely as a kind<br />

of Rorschach inkblot into which researchers project their particular<br />

biases <strong>and</strong> discern any interpretation that suits their fancy. Hunches<br />

may be gleaned, but hypotheses remain untested.<br />

Scarr-Salapatek searched through the records of a total of<br />

250,258 children from kindergarten to twelfth grade in the<br />

Philadelphia schools <strong>and</strong> found 1,521 twin pairs (493 of opposite<br />

sex <strong>and</strong> 1,028 of same sex). The racial distribution of the twin<br />

sample was 36 percent white <strong>and</strong> 64 percent Negro. <strong>Group</strong>administered<br />

IQ <strong>and</strong> scholastic achievement test scores were<br />

available. Scarr-Salapatek’s heritabilities in the white sample, both<br />

for IQ <strong>and</strong> scholastic achievement, are lower than the median<br />

values typically found in other studies of white populations in<br />

Engl<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> the United States. The heritability for both IQ <strong>and</strong><br />

achievement is slightly lower in Scarr-Salapatek’s Negro sample<br />

than in the w'hite, but the white <strong>and</strong> Negro heritabilities are about<br />

the same within social classes. In the Negro <strong>and</strong> in the white<br />

samples heritability increased as a function of social class, but<br />

the trend toward lower heritabilities for low SES holds only for<br />

the intelligence measures, while the achievement tends to go in

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!