06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Between-<strong>Group</strong>s Heritability 127<br />

The preference for environmental explanations of group differences<br />

reaches its zenith in a few studies. Gross (1967), for example,<br />

compared two Brooklyn Jewish groups on a variety of cognitive<br />

tests. The 90 Jewish boys, averaging about 6 years of age, came<br />

from either Sephardic families (immigrants from Arabic or Oriental<br />

countries) or Ashkenazic families (immigrants from Europe). All<br />

their mothers were native-born <strong>and</strong> English was the household<br />

language. All were middle class <strong>and</strong> lived in the same community.<br />

Yet the Ashkenazic boys scored higher than the Sephardic boys<br />

on the several cognitive tests <strong>and</strong> differed <strong>by</strong> as much as 17 IQ<br />

points on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Tests. The investigators<br />

studied the family environments intensively for clues that could<br />

explain the significant IQ difference between these two groups.<br />

No significant differences could be found between the groups in<br />

a host of family training <strong>and</strong> background experiences - except for<br />

one item in the questionnaire of parental attitudes. Twice as many<br />

Ashkenazic mothers said that earnings were ‘unimportant’ in their<br />

desires for their children, <strong>and</strong> three times as many Sephardic<br />

mothers said they wanted their sons to be ‘wealthy’. This single,<br />

subtle attitudinal factor, then, supposedly explains the 17 points<br />

IQ difference. There is no suggestion of the possibility that<br />

Sephardic <strong>and</strong> Ashkenazic groups may have different gene pools<br />

for many characteristics, including intelligence. The study is cited<br />

<strong>by</strong> other writers (e.g., Havighurst, 1970, p. 321) as an example of<br />

how subtle environmental differences can influence cognitive<br />

development. It is interesting that no one has produced IQ<br />

differences nearly as large as 17 points in non-disadvantaged groups<br />

even <strong>by</strong> the most intensive training. Direct coaching on a particular<br />

IQ test results in only about 9 or 10 points gain. Another zenith<br />

of environmentalism: as an example of the effects of environmental<br />

differences on IQ, Klineberg (1956) points to an IQ difference of<br />

47 points (58 v. 105)1 between a group of rural Negro children in<br />

Tennessee <strong>and</strong> a group of urban Negro children in Los Angeles.<br />

Since the largest difference ever reported between a pair of identical<br />

twins reared apart is 24 IQ points (Newman, Freeman &<br />

Holzinger, 1937), it seems most improbable that a mean difference<br />

of 47 IQ points would be attributable entirely to environmental<br />

differences. It is, of course, not impossible, but it is highly improbable.<br />

Preferences obviously do not always correspond to<br />

probabilities.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!