06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

76 <strong>Educability</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Group</strong> <strong>Differences</strong><br />

One of the most impressive characteristics of intelligence tests is<br />

the great diversity of means <strong>by</strong> which essentially the same ability<br />

can be measured. Tests having very diverse forms, such as vocabulary,<br />

block designs, matrices, number series, ‘odd man out’, figure<br />

copying, verbal analogies, <strong>and</strong> other kinds of problems can all<br />

serve as intelligence tests yielding more or less equivalent results<br />

because of their high intercorrelations. All of these types of tests<br />

have high loadings on the g factor, which, as Wechsler (1958, p.<br />

121) has said, ‘. . . involves broad mental organization; it is<br />

independent of the modality or contextual structure from which it<br />

is elicited; g cannot be exclusively identified with any single<br />

intellectual ability <strong>and</strong> for this reason cannot be described in<br />

concrete operational terms’. We can accurately define g only in<br />

terms of certain mathematical operations; in Wechsler’s words,<br />

lg is a measure of a collective communality which necessarily<br />

emerges from the intercorrelation of any broad sample of mental<br />

abilities’ (p. 123).<br />

Assessment of scholastic achievement, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, depends<br />

upon tests of narrowly specific acquired skills - reading, spelling,<br />

arithmetic operations, <strong>and</strong> the like. The forms <strong>by</strong> means of which<br />

one can test any one of these scholastic skills are very limited<br />

indeed. This is not to say that there is not a general factor common<br />

to all tests of scholastic achievement, but this general factor common<br />

to all the tests seems to be quite indistinguishable from the<br />

g factor of intelligence tests. Achievement tests, however, usually<br />

do not have quite as high g loadings as intelligence tests but<br />

have higher loadings on group factors such as verbal <strong>and</strong> numerical<br />

ability factors, containing, as well, more content-specific<br />

variance.<br />

It is always possible to make achievement tests correlate more<br />

highly with intelligence tests <strong>by</strong> requiring students to reason, to<br />

use data provided, <strong>and</strong> to apply their factual knowledge to the<br />

solution of new problems. More than just the mastery of factual<br />

information, intelligence is the ability to apply this information<br />

in new <strong>and</strong> different ways. With increasing grade level, achievement<br />

tests have more <strong>and</strong> more variance in common with tests of g.<br />

For example, once the basic skills in reading have been acquired,<br />

reading achievement tests must increasingly measure the student’s<br />

comprehension of more <strong>and</strong> more complex selections rather than<br />

the simpler processes of word recognition, decoding, etc. And thus

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!