06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

178 <strong>Educability</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Group</strong> <strong>Differences</strong><br />

evidence for it was the large IQ gains, amounting to an average of<br />

some 30 points, shown <strong>by</strong> severely deprived orphanage children,<br />

at about 18 months of age, when they were put into a more<br />

stimulating environment <strong>and</strong> were eventually reared in good<br />

adoptive homes (Skeels & Dye, 1939; Skeels, 1966). Bereiter argues<br />

that this magnitude of IQ gains is within the reaction range of<br />

IQ, assuming a heritability of 0-80 <strong>and</strong> equal additivity of environmental<br />

effects across the whole range of environments. But a<br />

30-point mean difference in group means suggests some individual<br />

changes outside the normal reaction range for IQ. In fact, four of<br />

the thirteen subjects in Skeels’ study showed IQ gains greater<br />

than 30 points (Skeels, 1966, Table 1). So perhaps the threshold<br />

hypothesis may be needed after all in order to account for these<br />

data.<br />

Probably the best evidence for the threshold hypothesis would<br />

be the finding of significantly higher heritability in groups that<br />

are above average in SES <strong>and</strong> environmental advantages than in<br />

groups of low SES.13 No one has ever done this systematically.<br />

The gifted children in Terman’s study came mostly from the<br />

higher SES levels <strong>and</strong> unquestionably had considerably better<br />

than average environmental advantages for intellectual development.<br />

The mean IQ of their siblings was 123 <strong>and</strong> the correlation<br />

between the IQs of the gifted <strong>and</strong> their siblings, estimated from<br />

the sibling regression, is 0-44, which, when corrected for attenuation,<br />

is close to the genetically predicted sibling correlations of<br />

0*5 (with r<strong>and</strong>om mating) or 0-6 (with an assortative mating<br />

coefficient of 0-5), <strong>and</strong> does not differ much from sibling correlations<br />

reported in the general literature. The gifted group as adults<br />

were, on the average, of higher SES than their own parents. Thus<br />

the offspring of the gifted probably enjoyed even greater environmental<br />

advantages. The narrow heritability of IQ in this group,<br />

estimated from the midparent-midchild regression, is 0-85. This<br />

is significantly higher than the best estimate of narrow' heritability<br />

(0*71) given <strong>by</strong> Jinks <strong>and</strong> Fulker (1970, p. 342) on the basis of<br />

Burt’s data, which includes a wride range of SES in the English<br />

population. It is also higher than the midparent-midchild correlation<br />

(0-69 + 0-03) found in a largely rural population sample in<br />

Vermont in 1920, with environmental advantages presumably<br />

much below those provided <strong>by</strong> the Terman gifted <strong>and</strong> their<br />

spouses (Jones, 1928, p. 69). These heritability findings, then, are

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!