06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Intelligence <strong>and</strong> <strong>Educability</strong> 77<br />

at higher grades, tests of reading comprehension, for those children<br />

who have already mastered the basic skills, become more or less<br />

indistinguishable in factorial composition from the so-called tests<br />

of verbal intelligence. Similarly, tests of mechanical arithmetic<br />

arithmetic computation) have less correlation with g than tests of<br />

arithmetic thought problems, such as the Arithmetic Concepts<br />

<strong>and</strong> Arithmetic Applications subtests of the Stanford Achievement<br />

battery. Accordingly, most indices of scholastic performance<br />

increasingly reflect general intelligence as children progress in<br />

school. We found in one study, for example, that up to grade 6,<br />

verbal <strong>and</strong> non-verbal intelligence tests could be factorially separated,<br />

with the scholastic achievement tests lining up on the same<br />

factor with verbal intelligence (<strong>Jensen</strong>, 1971a). But beyond grade 6<br />

both the verbal <strong>and</strong> non-verbal tests, along with all the scholastic<br />

achievement tests, amalgamated into a single large general factor<br />

which no form of factor rotation could separate into smaller<br />

components distinguishable as verbal intelligence v. non-verbal<br />

intelligence v. scholastic achievement. By grades 7 <strong>and</strong> 8 the<br />

Lorge-Thorndike Non-verbal IQ <strong>and</strong> Raven’s Progressive Matrices<br />

are hardly distinguishable in their factor composition from the<br />

tests of scholastic achievement. At the same time it is important to<br />

recognize that the Lorge-Thorndike Non-verbal IQ <strong>and</strong> Raven’s<br />

Matrices are not measuring scholastic attainment per se, as<br />

demonstrated <strong>by</strong> the fact that totally illiterate <strong>and</strong> unschooled<br />

persons can obtain high scores on these tests. Burt (1961a), for<br />

example, reported the case of separated identical twins with widely<br />

differing educational attainments (elementary school education v.<br />

a university degree), who differed <strong>by</strong> only one IQ point on the<br />

Progressive Matrices (127 v. 128).<br />

Another important characteristic of the best intelligence test<br />

items is that they clearly fall along an age scale. Items are thus<br />

‘naturally’ ordered in difficulty. The Figure Copying Test is a<br />

good example. The Figure Copying Test was developed at the<br />

Gesell Institute of Child Study at Yale University as a means for<br />

measuring developmental readiness for the traditional school<br />

learning tasks of the primary grades. The test consists of the<br />

ten geometric forms shown in Figure 3.1, arranged in order of<br />

difficulty, which the child must simply copy, each on a separate<br />

sheet of paper. The test involves no memory factor, since the figure<br />

to be copied is before the child at all times. It is administered

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!