06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

74 <strong>Educability</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Group</strong> <strong>Differences</strong><br />

presumably the same for the knowledge <strong>and</strong> skills sampled <strong>by</strong><br />

intelligence tests as for those sampled <strong>by</strong> scholastic achievement<br />

tests. In a sense, when we use intelligence tests to predict scholastic<br />

achievement, what we are doing is using achievement in one<br />

domain (non-scholastic) to predict achievement in another<br />

(scholastic).<br />

The most obvious difference between tests of intelligence <strong>and</strong> of<br />

achievement is the breadth of the domains sampled <strong>by</strong> the tests.<br />

Achievement tests sample very narrowly from the most specifically<br />

taught skills in the traditional curriculum, emphasizing the 3 Rs.<br />

The test items are samples of the particular skills <strong>and</strong> items of<br />

information that children are specifically taught in school. Since<br />

this domain is quite explicitly defined <strong>and</strong> the criteria of its<br />

acquisition are fairly clear to teachers <strong>and</strong> parents, children can<br />

be taught <strong>and</strong> made to practise these skills so as to shape their<br />

performance up to the desired st<strong>and</strong>ard. Because of the circumscribed<br />

nature of many of the basic scholastic skills, the pupil’s<br />

specific weaknesses can be identified <strong>and</strong> remedied.<br />

The kinds of skills <strong>and</strong> learning sampled <strong>by</strong> an intelligence test,<br />

on the other h<strong>and</strong>, represent achievements of a much broader<br />

nature. Much of what is tapped <strong>by</strong> IQ tests is acquired <strong>by</strong> incidental<br />

learning, that is to say, it has never been explicitly taught. Most<br />

of the words in a person’s vocabulary were never explicitly taught<br />

or acquired <strong>by</strong> studying a dictionary. Intelligence test items<br />

typically are sampled from such a wide range of potential experiences<br />

that the idea of teaching intelligence, as compared with<br />

teaching, say, reading <strong>and</strong> arithmetic, is practically nonsensical.<br />

Even direct coaching <strong>and</strong> practice on a particular intelligence test<br />

raises an individual’s scores on a parallel form of the test on the<br />

average <strong>by</strong> only five to ten points; <strong>and</strong> some tests, especially those<br />

referred to as ‘culture fair’, seem to be hardly amenable to the<br />

effects of coaching <strong>and</strong> practice. The average five-year-old child,<br />

for example, can copy a circle or a square without any trouble,<br />

but try to teach him to copy a diamond <strong>and</strong> see how far he gets!<br />

It is practically impossible. But wait until he is seven years old<br />

<strong>and</strong> he will have no trouble copying the diamond, without any<br />

need for instruction. Even vocabulary is very unsusceptible to<br />

enlargement <strong>by</strong> direct practice aimed specifically at increasing<br />

vocabulary.<br />

This is mainly the reason that vocabulary tests are such good

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!