06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

314 <strong>Educability</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Group</strong> <strong>Differences</strong><br />

‘bigger’, he actually believes the quantity of clay has been increased;<br />

<strong>and</strong> similarly when he sees liquid poured from a shallow,<br />

broad bowl into a tall, slender flask. There are many ways that<br />

the concept of conservation shows up: in number, length, area,<br />

time, weight, volume, <strong>and</strong> so on. Piaget has invented means for<br />

assessing children’s conservation concepts in all these forms, along<br />

with many other tests <strong>and</strong> procedures for studying the sequence<br />

of mental development throughout each of its main stages.<br />

Now, what have child psychologists learned from the application<br />

of Piaget’s tests that is relevant to Voyat’s commentary<br />

First, Voyat is probably correct in his opinion that the Piagetian<br />

tests are less culture-bound than conventional IQ tests. For one<br />

thing, some groups reared under environmental conditions which<br />

are extremely different from those of Western culture have been<br />

found to show not only the same sequence of development through<br />

Piaget’s stages, but are even somewhat more accelerated in this<br />

development than white middle-class children. Again, Arctic<br />

Eskimos were found to excel over white urban Canadian children<br />

in the Piagetian tests, <strong>and</strong> Canadian Indians do almost as well as<br />

the Eskimos (Vernon, 1965b; Mac<strong>Arthur</strong>, 1968, p. 48). Obviously<br />

it is not necessary to have lived in a Western or middle-class<br />

culture in order to perform up to Western middle-class levels on<br />

Piagetian tests.<br />

In rank-ordering children of the same chronological age in<br />

terms of their rate of mental development, the Piagetian tests are<br />

not very different from other culture-reduced tests. Vernon (1965b)<br />

factor analyzed a large number of Piagetian tests along with<br />

conventional psychometric measures of intelligence <strong>and</strong> found<br />

that the Piagetian tests were heavily loaded on g, the general<br />

factor common to all intelligence tests. In fact, the Piagetian tests<br />

measured little else than g; the non-g variance seems to be taskspecific,<br />

i.e., it has nothing in common with other Piagetian tests<br />

or with conventional IQ tests. Tuddenham (1970) gave a battery<br />

of Piagetian tests, along with Raven’s Matrices <strong>and</strong> the Peabody<br />

Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT), to a large number of elementary<br />

school children, <strong>and</strong> concluded: ‘. . . the Raven has the higher<br />

correlations, ranging from 0-24 to 0-50, as compared with Peabody<br />

values of 0-13 to 0-37 for a similar though not identical set of<br />

Piagetian items’ (p. 68). These are relatively high values for single<br />

item correlations within a restricted age range. Tuddenham notes

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!