06.02.2015 Views

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

Educability-and-Group-Differences-1973-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

324 <strong>Educability</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Group</strong> <strong>Differences</strong><br />

variable, the less likely we ought to expect relationship to school<br />

success. . . . As it is, there is good reason to think that school<br />

achievement is a result of a host of variables in addition to cognitive<br />

variables’ (p. 51). It seems likely, however, that Bosco<br />

underestimates the relevance of his measure of scholastic performance.<br />

This could be tested <strong>by</strong> determining correlations at<br />

the first grade, <strong>and</strong> <strong>by</strong> increasing the information load of the task<br />

at sixth grade in order to yield sufficient variance to permit<br />

significant correlations to show up.<br />

REACTION TIME<br />

Reaction time (RT) to a stimulus situation increases as the amount<br />

of information transmitted <strong>by</strong> the stimulus increases. RT increases<br />

as a linear function of ‘bits’ of information, <strong>and</strong> thus the rate of<br />

increase in RT can be taken as a measure of information processing<br />

capacity. (It has been reported also that the slope of this function<br />

is negatively correlated with IQ [Roth, 1964, cited in Eysenck,<br />

1967].) A description of one experimental procedure for demonstrating<br />

this measure will help to make it clear. The subject sits<br />

in front of a panel on which there is a single light bulb; directly<br />

beneath the bulb is a pushbutton. When the light flashes ‘on’, the<br />

subject pushes the button to turn the light ‘off’. In this condition,<br />

the subject’s response time is a measure of simple RT. There is<br />

zero information conveyed when there is only one light/button<br />

combination. But the subject is required to respond to an increasing<br />

number of light/button combinations, simply <strong>by</strong> having one<br />

light go on among an increasing number of potential alternatives.<br />

This is called ‘choice RT’. The amount of information conveyed<br />

increases logarithmically as the number of lights increases.<br />

Fox <strong>and</strong> Taylor (1967) compared two groups of army recruits<br />

on simple RT <strong>and</strong> choice RT. One group (low AFQT) was selected<br />

from recruits having scores between 10 <strong>and</strong> 21 on the Armed<br />

Forces Qualification Test (a composite measure of general<br />

intelligence <strong>and</strong> basic scholastic attainments); the other group<br />

(high AFQT) were recruits with scores from 90 to 99. The groups<br />

differed significantly in choice RT, but not in simple RT. (A<br />

more detailed description of the apparatus, procedure <strong>and</strong> results<br />

of this experiment is presented in <strong>Jensen</strong>, 1970b, pp. 149-51.)<br />

Also, Oswald (1971) found a correlation (r = —0-41) between

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!