28.11.2012 Views

IN THE BUBBLE JOHN THACKARA - witz cultural

IN THE BUBBLE JOHN THACKARA - witz cultural

IN THE BUBBLE JOHN THACKARA - witz cultural

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

180 Chapter 8<br />

make us switch off than on—especially if those voices are harbingers of bad<br />

news about the environment. The same caveat applies if we feed the data<br />

from remote environmental sensing into fancy interactive displays.<br />

A backlash against interactive displays in museums and science centers is<br />

already evident. Armando Iannucci, a British writer, took his children to<br />

the Natural History Museum in London; he concluded that ‘‘interactivity<br />

is a superficial sham leading only to hunger and emptiness. The relentless,<br />

raucous, lapel-grabbing interactivity of the newer displays was so offputting.<br />

The display cases invited participation in a process, only to lead<br />

you to the conclusion that the process was not worth pursuing. Interactivity<br />

implies participation—but does not deliver it.’’ 58 Dwell time at an interactive<br />

exhibit at the average science center is forty seconds.<br />

Don’t Look, Talk<br />

‘‘All knowledge is dialogic,’’ said the theologian Martin Büber. 59 Maybe we<br />

just need to talk to other human beings more—face to face. It sounds trite,<br />

but for thousands of years, talk was one of the main ways that humans<br />

tried to understand and influence the world around them. Then came media.<br />

Ivan Illich discovered that in the 1930s, nine out of ten words that a<br />

man had heard when he reached the age of twenty were words spoken to<br />

him directly—one to one, or as a member of a crowd—by somebody<br />

whom he could touch and feel and smell. By the 1970s, that proportion<br />

had been reversed: About nine out of ten words heard in a day were spoken<br />

through a loudspeaker. ‘‘Computers are doing to communication what<br />

fences did to pastures and cars did to streets,’’ Illich said in 1982. 60 For<br />

Illich, there was a huge difference between a colloquial tongue—what people<br />

say to each other in a context, with meaning—and a language uttered<br />

by people into microphones.<br />

Theodor Zeldin, who has written a book about conversation, believes<br />

that conversation can penetrate the intellectual barriers that are often associated<br />

with specialized professions. ‘‘The really big scientific revolutions<br />

have been the invention not of some new machine, but of new ways of<br />

talking about things,’’ argues Zeldin, adding, ‘‘what we need now is stimulation,<br />

not information.’’ 61 Thirty-five years after Illich’s comments about<br />

conversation, Zeldin’s book has clearly struck a chord; one reviewer said

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!