08.09.2014 Views

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Assisting</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>older</strong> <strong>driver</strong><br />

persons scoring relatively poor on <strong>the</strong>m were considered to be functionally<br />

<strong>older</strong>.<br />

To summarize expectations, functionally <strong>older</strong> <strong>driver</strong>s were expected to have<br />

more difficulties crossing complex intersections at which <strong>the</strong>y have to decide<br />

how to deal with o<strong>the</strong>r road users than functionally younger <strong>driver</strong>s do. We<br />

expected that <strong>the</strong>se differences in difficulties would be reflected in<br />

differences in experienced workload and safety of <strong>driver</strong> decisions. In<br />

addition, we expected an intersection to be more complex a) if it was<br />

connecting more streets and traffic lanes, b) if <strong>the</strong>re was no o<strong>the</strong>r priority<br />

regulation than right goes before left, and c) if <strong>the</strong> view of <strong>the</strong> intersection<br />

was blocked by cars, buildings or trees. Knowing which design elements of<br />

intersections are responsible for task demands that overask <strong>the</strong> capabilities of<br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>older</strong> <strong>driver</strong> will give road authorities <strong>the</strong> opportunity to adapt <strong>the</strong><br />

infrastructure to <strong>the</strong> demands of tomorrow’s <strong>driver</strong> population.<br />

6.2. Method<br />

6.2.1. Participants<br />

Participants were recruited by means of a newspaper article about <strong>the</strong><br />

driving simulator, and by advertisements in supermarkets. Participants had<br />

to be ei<strong>the</strong>r between 30 and 50 years of age or 70 years of age or <strong>older</strong>. Thirtythree<br />

<strong>older</strong> adult <strong>driver</strong>s (70 to 88 years of age; μ = 75.2, σ = 4.8; 26 males, 7<br />

females) and seventy‐two younger <strong>driver</strong>s (28 to 51 years of age; μ = 39.2, σ =<br />

6.6; 41 males, 31 females) participated in this study. All participants had at<br />

least 5 years of driving experience and passed a test on sensitivity to<br />

simulator sickness (Motion Sickness/Simulator Sickness Screening Form<br />

MSSF; see Hoffman, Molino & Inman, 2003).<br />

After being selected based on <strong>the</strong>ir chronological age, participants were<br />

grouped according to <strong>the</strong>ir functional age. Functional age was based on<br />

scores on three tests of cognitive functioning, relating to reaction time,<br />

selective attention, and visual‐motor coordination (Determination Test © S1<br />

[Schuhfried, 2003], Tachistoscopic Traffic Test Mannheim for Screen<br />

TAVTMB © S1 [Biehl, 2003], and adaptive tracking task [see e.g., Ponds,<br />

Brouwer & Van Wolffelaar, 1988; Withaar & Brouwer, 2003] respectively).<br />

We used <strong>the</strong> median reaction time provided by <strong>the</strong> Determination Test © , <strong>the</strong><br />

overview score of <strong>the</strong> TAVTMB © and <strong>the</strong> performance on <strong>the</strong> second trial<br />

(third trial when considering <strong>the</strong> practice trial) of <strong>the</strong> adaptive tracking task.<br />

106

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!