08.09.2014 Views

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Assisting</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>older</strong> <strong>driver</strong><br />

In <strong>the</strong> present study, <strong>the</strong> functions of <strong>the</strong> <strong>driver</strong> information system of<br />

Entenmann and Küting (2001) were combined with <strong>the</strong> ‘gap selecting’‐<br />

function tested in <strong>the</strong> EDDIT‐project. A <strong>driver</strong> assistance system was tested<br />

which not only provided <strong>the</strong> <strong>driver</strong> with information on speed limits, oneway<br />

streets, and priority regulations, but also actively warned <strong>the</strong> <strong>driver</strong> to<br />

reduce speed when approaching an intersection at which <strong>the</strong> sight on <strong>the</strong><br />

crossing street was obstructed by buildings, and which indicated when it<br />

was safe to join or cross traffic streams at busy intersections. Analogous to<br />

<strong>the</strong> idea of Entenmann and Küting, this assistance system was intended to<br />

reduce workload at <strong>the</strong> intersection by timely informing <strong>the</strong> <strong>driver</strong> about<br />

what to expect. If <strong>the</strong> <strong>driver</strong> ei<strong>the</strong>r consciously or subconsciously knows<br />

what to expect, it is more likely that he will select <strong>the</strong> correct mental schema<br />

for safely passing <strong>the</strong> intersection. This mental schema will simplify <strong>the</strong><br />

driving task and will prevent errors. The present study was designed to<br />

evaluate <strong>the</strong> <strong>driver</strong> assistance system in this respect. The system was<br />

evaluated in terms of effects on workload, driving performance, and user<br />

acceptance.<br />

While evaluating <strong>the</strong> system, <strong>the</strong> effects on driving behaviour were<br />

compared between <strong>older</strong> (70 years and <strong>older</strong>) and younger persons (30‐50<br />

years old). The age boundaries of <strong>the</strong> first group were chosen as a<br />

compromise between <strong>the</strong> preferred group of people aged 75 and above – <strong>the</strong><br />

group of <strong>older</strong> adults that has a fatality rate which is higher than average<br />

(OECD, 2001; <strong>SWOV</strong>, 2005) – and <strong>the</strong> availability of participants of a certain<br />

age. The group of people aged 30‐50 was chosen as <strong>the</strong> comparison group.<br />

People of this age group were expected to have considerable driving<br />

experience and not yet to be confronted with functional limitations. As <strong>the</strong><br />

information provided by our information system was selected based on <strong>the</strong><br />

functional limitations of <strong>older</strong> <strong>driver</strong>s, it was expected that <strong>older</strong> <strong>driver</strong>s<br />

would benefit more from <strong>the</strong> information given in terms of reduced<br />

workload and, as a result, would also be more willing to accept <strong>the</strong> system.<br />

Realizing that ageing is a process that does not start at <strong>the</strong> same age for every<br />

person nor continues at <strong>the</strong> same rate, with <strong>the</strong> result that <strong>driver</strong>s having <strong>the</strong><br />

same age (as in years since birth) can differ in <strong>the</strong> number and severity of<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir functional limitations, age was both considered chronologically and<br />

functionally (for a discussion on <strong>the</strong> prediction of functional age see Birren &<br />

Renner, 1977, p. 15‐17). Functional age was based on scores on three tests of<br />

cognitive functioning, relating to reaction time, selective attention, and<br />

visual‐motor coordination. Persons scoring well on <strong>the</strong> average of <strong>the</strong>se three<br />

154

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!