08.09.2014 Views

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Summary, discussion and general conclusions<br />

Some of <strong>the</strong>se findings are not consistent with those of o<strong>the</strong>r studies (e.g., De<br />

Waard, Van der Hulst, & Brookhuis, 1999; Viborg, 1999). As discussed in<br />

Chapter 8, this can be caused by differences in <strong>the</strong> types of messages which<br />

were sent to <strong>the</strong> <strong>driver</strong>s (driving too fast vs. driving too slow) and by<br />

regression to <strong>the</strong> mean.<br />

As regards <strong>the</strong> willingness to buy <strong>the</strong> system that was “installed” in <strong>the</strong><br />

simulator car, 35% of <strong>the</strong> participants indicated that <strong>the</strong>y were willing to buy<br />

<strong>the</strong> system if it were on <strong>the</strong> market. Half of <strong>the</strong>m was willing to pay for it.<br />

Most participants were positive about <strong>the</strong> audibility and comprehensibility<br />

of <strong>the</strong> messages. Except for one participant, everyone was very well able to<br />

hear <strong>the</strong> messages. Eighty‐five per cent of <strong>the</strong> participants indicated that <strong>the</strong><br />

messages were also easy to understand and that it was easy to act upon<br />

<strong>the</strong>m. This does not mean that <strong>the</strong>y always did act upon <strong>the</strong>m. Only 18% said<br />

that <strong>the</strong>y always followed <strong>the</strong> advice of <strong>the</strong> support system. Ano<strong>the</strong>r 63%<br />

frequently followed <strong>the</strong> system’s advice. One of <strong>the</strong> reasons for not following<br />

<strong>the</strong> system’s advices may be that participants did not always trust <strong>the</strong>m.<br />

Thirty‐five per cent of <strong>the</strong> participants trusted <strong>the</strong> advices only occasionally<br />

or never. Two participants indicated that it depended on <strong>the</strong> type of<br />

messages; <strong>the</strong>y always trusted information about traffic rules, but never<br />

about safe gaps to join or cross.<br />

Summary of main findings<br />

As regards <strong>the</strong> safety of <strong>older</strong> <strong>driver</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> Ne<strong>the</strong>rlands, it was found that:<br />

• The fatality rate of <strong>driver</strong>s aged 75 and above is <strong>the</strong> largest of all <strong>driver</strong>s<br />

in <strong>the</strong> Ne<strong>the</strong>rlands.<br />

• The high fatality rate of <strong>older</strong> <strong>driver</strong>s is mainly <strong>the</strong> result of <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

increased physical vulnerability.<br />

• Older <strong>driver</strong>s are over‐represented in crashes at intersections and more<br />

often legally responsible for <strong>the</strong>se crashes, often because <strong>the</strong>y fail to<br />

yield. These crashes particularly occur when <strong>the</strong> <strong>older</strong> <strong>driver</strong> has to turn<br />

left.<br />

• In general, <strong>the</strong>re are few indications that a decline in visual and<br />

cognitive functions as part of normal ageing has negative road safety<br />

consequences. Most <strong>driver</strong>s can compensate for <strong>the</strong>se declines. In case<br />

of moderate or severe functional limitations due to age‐related<br />

disorders and diseases, <strong>older</strong> <strong>driver</strong>s need more time to prepare and<br />

execute a driving manoeuvre and have more difficulties performing<br />

different activities in parallel.<br />

201

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!