08.09.2014 Views

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Assisting</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>older</strong> <strong>driver</strong><br />

support was available, and 3) <strong>the</strong> frequency at which <strong>the</strong> stimuli were<br />

presented was every 3.5 s instead of every 4 s. Practise on <strong>the</strong> PDT was<br />

provided during <strong>the</strong> familiarization run.<br />

6.3. Results<br />

In this section, between‐subjects comparisons focus on functional age.<br />

Within‐subjects comparisons focus on <strong>the</strong> effects of different types of<br />

intersections and manoeuvres.<br />

6.3.1. General driving performance<br />

Waiting position before passing an intersection, and acceleration and speed<br />

while passing an intersection were averaged per required <strong>driver</strong> manoeuvre:<br />

crossing a street while driving straight ahead, joining traffic after turning left<br />

or right, or turning left in front of an approaching vehicle. Age groups only<br />

differed in <strong>the</strong>ir waiting position before entering <strong>the</strong> intersection.<br />

Functionally old participants generally stopped two meters fur<strong>the</strong>r away<br />

from <strong>the</strong> intersection to decide whe<strong>the</strong>r it was safe to join traffic after turning<br />

left or right than both young and middle‐aged participants did (t(18) = ‐<br />

3.188, p = 0.005 and t(28) = ‐ 2.739, p = 0.011 respectively). In addition, while<br />

stopping in front of <strong>the</strong> intersection before crossing it, functionally old<br />

participants had a lateral position that was closer to <strong>the</strong> side of <strong>the</strong> road than<br />

both young and middle‐aged participants had (t(11) = 2.405, p = 0.036 and<br />

t(12) = 2.840, p = 0.015 respectively). Acceleration and speed while passing<br />

intersections did not differ between age groups.<br />

6.3.2. Workload<br />

Before any of <strong>the</strong> analyses with regard to workload were carried out, all<br />

average reaction times – those averaged per intersection as well as those<br />

averaged per baseline – were first corrected for missing data. Initially,<br />

average reaction times represented <strong>the</strong> average of <strong>the</strong> reaction times for those<br />

stimuli to which participants had reacted within 2.5 s. If a participant had not<br />

reacted within 2.5 s after <strong>the</strong> onset of a stimulus, <strong>the</strong> reaction time for that<br />

stimulus was coded as missing, and <strong>the</strong> corresponding average reaction time<br />

per intersection or baseline was based on a lower number of stimuli. A<br />

reason for not having reacted within 2.5 s could ei<strong>the</strong>r be not having noticed<br />

<strong>the</strong> stimulus or not having reacted quickly enough. In both cases, no<br />

attentional capacity was left to notice and or react to <strong>the</strong> stimulus in time.<br />

Had <strong>the</strong> stimulus been presented for a longer span of time, or had we waited<br />

114

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!