08.09.2014 Views

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

Assisting the older driver - SWOV

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Assisting</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>older</strong> <strong>driver</strong><br />

sensitive to ageing. Tests in <strong>the</strong>se domains have been repeatedly found to<br />

correlate with poor on‐road driving performance and crash involvement (see<br />

e.g., Ball et al., 2006; Hoffman, McDowd, Atchley & Dubinsky, 2005; Wood,<br />

2002).<br />

Dynamic visual acuity and detection of movement<br />

Dynamic visual acuity is <strong>the</strong> ability to resolve details of a moving target. Its<br />

deterioration is probably attributable to <strong>the</strong> required oculomotor control<br />

(Shinar & Schieber, 1991). The oculomotor system is also involved in<br />

detection of movement. However, <strong>the</strong> decline in detection of movement with<br />

increasing age appears to be mainly <strong>the</strong> result of age‐related changes to<br />

neural mechanisms. Obviously, <strong>the</strong> ability to detect movement is very<br />

important for safe driving, not only for being able to detect vehicles driving<br />

on an intersecting road and to estimate <strong>the</strong>ir speed, but also for being able to<br />

detect changes in <strong>the</strong> speed of vehicles in front, i.e., stopping, slowing down,<br />

speeding up, and reversing (Holland, 2001; Shinar & Schieber, 1991).<br />

Night‐time visual acuity and sensitivity to glare<br />

Impaired night‐time visual acuity is <strong>the</strong> result of two age‐related changes<br />

that reduce <strong>the</strong> amount of light reaching <strong>the</strong> retina: reduced pupil size and<br />

yellowing of <strong>the</strong> lens (Olson, 1993). A consequence of reduced retinal<br />

illumination is that sources must be of higher intensity to be seen at night<br />

(e.g., Olson, 1993). Sensitivity to glare, which increases between <strong>the</strong> ages of<br />

40 and 70, leads to a slower recovery from headlights and o<strong>the</strong>r reflecting<br />

sources (Fozard et al. (1977) cited in Aizenberg & McKenzie, 1997).<br />

Contrast sensitivity<br />

As far as contrast sensitivity is concerned, <strong>older</strong> adults have more difficulties<br />

detecting objects that are not outlined clearly or that do not stand out from<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir background. Its deterioration is probably attributable to changes in <strong>the</strong><br />

eye itself as well as neural factors. Contrast sensitivity is – even more than<br />

visual acuity – necessary for <strong>the</strong> perception of (<strong>the</strong> information on) traffic<br />

signs. Besides this, contrast sensitivity is also believed to play a role in<br />

distance perception and <strong>the</strong> estimation of <strong>the</strong> speed of moving objects<br />

(Holland, 2001; Shinar & Schieber, 1991).<br />

2.2.2. Cognitive functions<br />

Age‐related declines in sensory functions such as vision and hearing have an<br />

impact on <strong>the</strong> input <strong>the</strong> <strong>driver</strong> receives from o<strong>the</strong>r road users and from <strong>the</strong><br />

road environment (e.g., traffic signs and signals, road markings). To select<br />

36

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!