26.01.2013 Views

Presuppositions in Spoken Discourse

Presuppositions in Spoken Discourse

Presuppositions in Spoken Discourse

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Chapter 4<br />

None of the triggered presuppositions had b<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g as the most frequent<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretation. <strong>Presuppositions</strong> triggered by def<strong>in</strong>ite NPs had antecedents <strong>in</strong> around<br />

30% of the cases for both annotators. 6 That the primary usage of def<strong>in</strong>ite NPs is<br />

not anaphoric reference to someth<strong>in</strong>g already given <strong>in</strong> the text has been established<br />

<strong>in</strong> earlier corpus work of written Swedish by Fraurud (1990) and more recently with<br />

texts from the Wall Street Journal <strong>in</strong> an annotation task experiment done by Poesio<br />

& Vieira (1998). The figures here confirm this for unrestricted spoken dialogue.<br />

The frequency of b<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g for factives and aspectual verbs was very low. The<br />

triggers most likely to be resolved by b<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g are it-clefts and too. In fact, the<br />

presuppositions <strong>in</strong>duced by too were almost exclusively resolved by b<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g with a<br />

discourse-given antecedent.<br />

Induced presuppositions that are <strong>in</strong>terpreted as bound are meant to be<br />

referr<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>formation already <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>in</strong> the discourse. The most basic<br />

requirement placed on a potential antecedent is that the <strong>in</strong>formation it contributes<br />

is judged as compatible with the semantic content of the presupposed material.<br />

However, the more descriptive <strong>in</strong>formation associated with the triggered<br />

presupposition, the more difficult mak<strong>in</strong>g this judgment becomes. For triggers that<br />

semantically presuppose an <strong>in</strong>dividual, we must try to f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> our discourse<br />

representation an <strong>in</strong>dividual reference marker that has compatible semantic<br />

features. But for propositions, events, facts, and situations, e.g. abstract objects,<br />

identify<strong>in</strong>g a compatible antecedent is a much harder task. In practice this means<br />

6 Note that <strong>in</strong> this table the def<strong>in</strong>ite NPs that were considered bridg<strong>in</strong>g references are counted<br />

separately, and are treated <strong>in</strong> more detail <strong>in</strong> chapter 6. This means that the percentage of e.g.<br />

33% b<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g for def<strong>in</strong>ite NPs for the first annotator does not mean 67% accommodation. For<br />

the figures for accommodation see Table 2 or Table 5.<br />

68<br />

Trigger<strong>in</strong>g<br />

construction<br />

TOTAL B<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g<br />

factive verbs,–obj. comp. 109 28 (26%)<br />

aspectual verbs 68 10 (15%)<br />

it-clefts 31 19 (61%)<br />

too 45 43 (98%)<br />

Table 3 Cases of b<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g, abstract triggers<br />

Trigger<strong>in</strong>g<br />

construction<br />

TOTAL B<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g<br />

2 annotators 1st 2nd 1st 2nd<br />

all def<strong>in</strong>ite NPs 411 390 137(33%) 113(30%)<br />

def<strong>in</strong>ite NPs 248 235 78(31%) 67(28%)<br />

demonstrative NPs 53 47 27(510%) 16(34%)<br />

possessives 110 108 32(29%) 30(28%)<br />

Table 4 Cases of b<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g - def<strong>in</strong>ite descriptions

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!